Cargando…
The hands’ default location guides tactile spatial selectivity
Our skin is a two-dimensional sheet that can be folded into a multitude of configurations due to the mobility of our body parts. Parts of the human tactile system might account for this flexibility by being tuned to locations in the world rather than on the skin. Using adaptation, we scrutinized the...
Autores principales: | , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
National Academy of Sciences
2023
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10104573/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/37014855 http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2209680120 |
_version_ | 1785026067624361984 |
---|---|
author | Badde, Stephanie Heed, Tobias |
author_facet | Badde, Stephanie Heed, Tobias |
author_sort | Badde, Stephanie |
collection | PubMed |
description | Our skin is a two-dimensional sheet that can be folded into a multitude of configurations due to the mobility of our body parts. Parts of the human tactile system might account for this flexibility by being tuned to locations in the world rather than on the skin. Using adaptation, we scrutinized the spatial selectivity of two tactile perceptual mechanisms for which the visual equivalents have been reported to be selective in world coordinates: tactile motion and the duration of tactile events. Participants’ hand position—uncrossed or crossed—as well as the stimulated hand varied independently across adaptation and test phases. This design distinguished among somatotopic selectivity for locations on the skin and spatiotopic selectivity for locations in the environment, but also tested spatial selectivity that fits neither of these classical reference frames and is based on the default position of the hands. For both features, adaptation consistently affected subsequent tactile perception at the adapted hand, reflecting skin-bound spatial selectivity. Yet, tactile motion and temporal adaptation also transferred across hands but only if the hands were crossed during the adaptation phase, that is, when one hand was placed at the other hand’s typical location. Thus, selectivity for locations in the world was based on default rather than online sensory information about the location of the hands. These results challenge the prevalent dichotomy of somatotopic and spatiotopic selectivity and suggest that prior information about the hands’ default position —right hand at the right side—is embedded deep in the tactile sensory system. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-10104573 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2023 |
publisher | National Academy of Sciences |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-101045732023-04-15 The hands’ default location guides tactile spatial selectivity Badde, Stephanie Heed, Tobias Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A Biological Sciences Our skin is a two-dimensional sheet that can be folded into a multitude of configurations due to the mobility of our body parts. Parts of the human tactile system might account for this flexibility by being tuned to locations in the world rather than on the skin. Using adaptation, we scrutinized the spatial selectivity of two tactile perceptual mechanisms for which the visual equivalents have been reported to be selective in world coordinates: tactile motion and the duration of tactile events. Participants’ hand position—uncrossed or crossed—as well as the stimulated hand varied independently across adaptation and test phases. This design distinguished among somatotopic selectivity for locations on the skin and spatiotopic selectivity for locations in the environment, but also tested spatial selectivity that fits neither of these classical reference frames and is based on the default position of the hands. For both features, adaptation consistently affected subsequent tactile perception at the adapted hand, reflecting skin-bound spatial selectivity. Yet, tactile motion and temporal adaptation also transferred across hands but only if the hands were crossed during the adaptation phase, that is, when one hand was placed at the other hand’s typical location. Thus, selectivity for locations in the world was based on default rather than online sensory information about the location of the hands. These results challenge the prevalent dichotomy of somatotopic and spatiotopic selectivity and suggest that prior information about the hands’ default position —right hand at the right side—is embedded deep in the tactile sensory system. National Academy of Sciences 2023-04-04 2023-04-11 /pmc/articles/PMC10104573/ /pubmed/37014855 http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2209680120 Text en Copyright © 2023 the Author(s). Published by PNAS. https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/This open access article is distributed under Creative Commons Attribution License 4.0 (CC BY) (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) . |
spellingShingle | Biological Sciences Badde, Stephanie Heed, Tobias The hands’ default location guides tactile spatial selectivity |
title | The hands’ default location guides tactile spatial selectivity |
title_full | The hands’ default location guides tactile spatial selectivity |
title_fullStr | The hands’ default location guides tactile spatial selectivity |
title_full_unstemmed | The hands’ default location guides tactile spatial selectivity |
title_short | The hands’ default location guides tactile spatial selectivity |
title_sort | hands’ default location guides tactile spatial selectivity |
topic | Biological Sciences |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10104573/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/37014855 http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2209680120 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT baddestephanie thehandsdefaultlocationguidestactilespatialselectivity AT heedtobias thehandsdefaultlocationguidestactilespatialselectivity AT baddestephanie handsdefaultlocationguidestactilespatialselectivity AT heedtobias handsdefaultlocationguidestactilespatialselectivity |