Cargando…

Using Normative Language When Describing Scientific Findings: Randomized Controlled Trial of Effects on Trust and Credibility

BACKGROUND: Scientists often make cognitive claims (eg, the results of their work) and normative claims (eg, what should be done based on those results). Yet, these types of statements contain very different information and implications. This randomized controlled trial sought to characterize the gr...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Agley, Jon, Xiao, Yunyu, Thompson, Esi E, Golzarri-Arroyo, Lilian
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: JMIR Publications 2023
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10131812/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/36995753
http://dx.doi.org/10.2196/45482
_version_ 1785031258928054272
author Agley, Jon
Xiao, Yunyu
Thompson, Esi E
Golzarri-Arroyo, Lilian
author_facet Agley, Jon
Xiao, Yunyu
Thompson, Esi E
Golzarri-Arroyo, Lilian
author_sort Agley, Jon
collection PubMed
description BACKGROUND: Scientists often make cognitive claims (eg, the results of their work) and normative claims (eg, what should be done based on those results). Yet, these types of statements contain very different information and implications. This randomized controlled trial sought to characterize the granular effects of using normative language in science communication. OBJECTIVE: Our study examined whether viewing a social media post containing scientific claims about face masks for COVID-19 using both normative and cognitive language (intervention arm) would reduce perceptions of trust and credibility in science and scientists compared with an identical post using only cognitive language (control arm). We also examined whether effects were mediated by political orientation. METHODS: This was a 2-arm, parallel group, randomized controlled trial. We aimed to recruit 1500 US adults (age 18+) from the Prolific platform who were representative of the US population census by cross sections of age, race/ethnicity, and gender. Participants were randomly assigned to view 1 of 2 images of a social media post about face masks to prevent COVID-19. The control image described the results of a real study (cognitive language), and the intervention image was identical, but also included recommendations from the same study about what people should do based on the results (normative language). Primary outcomes were trust in science and scientists (21-item scale) and 4 individual items related to trust and credibility; 9 additional covariates (eg, sociodemographics, political orientation) were measured and included in analyses. RESULTS: From September 4, 2022, to September 6, 2022, 1526 individuals completed the study. For the sample as a whole (eg, without interaction terms), there was no evidence that a single exposure to normative language affected perceptions of trust or credibility in science or scientists. When including the interaction term (study arm × political orientation), there was some evidence of differential effects, such that individuals with liberal political orientation were more likely to trust scientific information from the social media post’s author if the post included normative language, and political conservatives were more likely to trust scientific information from the post’s author if the post included only cognitive language (β=0.05, 95% CI 0.00 to 0.10; P=.04). CONCLUSIONS: This study does not support the authors’ original hypotheses that single exposures to normative language can reduce perceptions of trust or credibility in science or scientists for all people. However, the secondary preregistered analyses indicate the possibility that political orientation may differentially mediate the effect of normative and cognitive language from scientists on people’s perceptions. We do not submit this paper as definitive evidence thereof but do believe that there is sufficient evidence to support additional research into this topic, which may have implications for effective scientific communication. TRIAL REGISTRATION: OSF Registries osf.io/kb3yh; https://osf.io/kb3yh INTERNATIONAL REGISTERED REPORT IDENTIFIER (IRRID): RR2-10.2196/41747
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-10131812
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2023
publisher JMIR Publications
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-101318122023-04-27 Using Normative Language When Describing Scientific Findings: Randomized Controlled Trial of Effects on Trust and Credibility Agley, Jon Xiao, Yunyu Thompson, Esi E Golzarri-Arroyo, Lilian J Med Internet Res Original Paper BACKGROUND: Scientists often make cognitive claims (eg, the results of their work) and normative claims (eg, what should be done based on those results). Yet, these types of statements contain very different information and implications. This randomized controlled trial sought to characterize the granular effects of using normative language in science communication. OBJECTIVE: Our study examined whether viewing a social media post containing scientific claims about face masks for COVID-19 using both normative and cognitive language (intervention arm) would reduce perceptions of trust and credibility in science and scientists compared with an identical post using only cognitive language (control arm). We also examined whether effects were mediated by political orientation. METHODS: This was a 2-arm, parallel group, randomized controlled trial. We aimed to recruit 1500 US adults (age 18+) from the Prolific platform who were representative of the US population census by cross sections of age, race/ethnicity, and gender. Participants were randomly assigned to view 1 of 2 images of a social media post about face masks to prevent COVID-19. The control image described the results of a real study (cognitive language), and the intervention image was identical, but also included recommendations from the same study about what people should do based on the results (normative language). Primary outcomes were trust in science and scientists (21-item scale) and 4 individual items related to trust and credibility; 9 additional covariates (eg, sociodemographics, political orientation) were measured and included in analyses. RESULTS: From September 4, 2022, to September 6, 2022, 1526 individuals completed the study. For the sample as a whole (eg, without interaction terms), there was no evidence that a single exposure to normative language affected perceptions of trust or credibility in science or scientists. When including the interaction term (study arm × political orientation), there was some evidence of differential effects, such that individuals with liberal political orientation were more likely to trust scientific information from the social media post’s author if the post included normative language, and political conservatives were more likely to trust scientific information from the post’s author if the post included only cognitive language (β=0.05, 95% CI 0.00 to 0.10; P=.04). CONCLUSIONS: This study does not support the authors’ original hypotheses that single exposures to normative language can reduce perceptions of trust or credibility in science or scientists for all people. However, the secondary preregistered analyses indicate the possibility that political orientation may differentially mediate the effect of normative and cognitive language from scientists on people’s perceptions. We do not submit this paper as definitive evidence thereof but do believe that there is sufficient evidence to support additional research into this topic, which may have implications for effective scientific communication. TRIAL REGISTRATION: OSF Registries osf.io/kb3yh; https://osf.io/kb3yh INTERNATIONAL REGISTERED REPORT IDENTIFIER (IRRID): RR2-10.2196/41747 JMIR Publications 2023-03-30 /pmc/articles/PMC10131812/ /pubmed/36995753 http://dx.doi.org/10.2196/45482 Text en ©Jon Agley, Yunyu Xiao, Esi E Thompson, Lilian Golzarri-Arroyo. Originally published in the Journal of Medical Internet Research (https://www.jmir.org), 30.03.2023. https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work, first published in the Journal of Medical Internet Research, is properly cited. The complete bibliographic information, a link to the original publication on https://www.jmir.org/, as well as this copyright and license information must be included.
spellingShingle Original Paper
Agley, Jon
Xiao, Yunyu
Thompson, Esi E
Golzarri-Arroyo, Lilian
Using Normative Language When Describing Scientific Findings: Randomized Controlled Trial of Effects on Trust and Credibility
title Using Normative Language When Describing Scientific Findings: Randomized Controlled Trial of Effects on Trust and Credibility
title_full Using Normative Language When Describing Scientific Findings: Randomized Controlled Trial of Effects on Trust and Credibility
title_fullStr Using Normative Language When Describing Scientific Findings: Randomized Controlled Trial of Effects on Trust and Credibility
title_full_unstemmed Using Normative Language When Describing Scientific Findings: Randomized Controlled Trial of Effects on Trust and Credibility
title_short Using Normative Language When Describing Scientific Findings: Randomized Controlled Trial of Effects on Trust and Credibility
title_sort using normative language when describing scientific findings: randomized controlled trial of effects on trust and credibility
topic Original Paper
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10131812/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/36995753
http://dx.doi.org/10.2196/45482
work_keys_str_mv AT agleyjon usingnormativelanguagewhendescribingscientificfindingsrandomizedcontrolledtrialofeffectsontrustandcredibility
AT xiaoyunyu usingnormativelanguagewhendescribingscientificfindingsrandomizedcontrolledtrialofeffectsontrustandcredibility
AT thompsonesie usingnormativelanguagewhendescribingscientificfindingsrandomizedcontrolledtrialofeffectsontrustandcredibility
AT golzarriarroyolilian usingnormativelanguagewhendescribingscientificfindingsrandomizedcontrolledtrialofeffectsontrustandcredibility