Cargando…
Deriving the number of salience maps an observer has from the number and quality of concurrent centroid judgments
[C. Koch, S. Ullman, Hum. Neurobiol.4, 219–227 (1985)] proposed a 2D topographical salience map that took feature-map outputs as its input and represented the importance “saliency” of the feature inputs at each location as a real number. The computation on the map, “winner-take-all,” was used to pre...
Autores principales: | , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
National Academy of Sciences
2023
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10214162/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/37186842 http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2301707120 |
_version_ | 1785145882855866368 |
---|---|
author | Gan, Lingyu Sun, Peng Sperling, George |
author_facet | Gan, Lingyu Sun, Peng Sperling, George |
author_sort | Gan, Lingyu |
collection | PubMed |
description | [C. Koch, S. Ullman, Hum. Neurobiol.4, 219–227 (1985)] proposed a 2D topographical salience map that took feature-map outputs as its input and represented the importance “saliency” of the feature inputs at each location as a real number. The computation on the map, “winner-take-all,” was used to predict action priority. We propose that the same or a similar map is used to compute centroid judgments, the center of a cloud of diverse items. [P. Sun, V. Chu, G. Sperling, Atten. Percept. Psychophys.83, 934–955 (2021)] demonstrated that following a 250-msec exposure of a 24-dot array of 3 intermixed colors, subjects could accurately report the centroid of each dot color, thereby indicating that these subjects had at least three salience maps. Here, we use a postcue, partial-report paradigm to determine how many more salience maps subjects might have. In 11 experiments, subjects viewed 0.3-s flashes of 28 to 32 item arrays composed of M, M = 3,...,8, different features followed by a cue to mouse-click the centroid of items of just the post-cued feature. Ideal detector response analyses show that subjects utilized at least 12 to 17 stimulus items. By determining whether a subject’s performance in (M-1)-feature experiments could/could-not predict performance in M-feature experiments, we conclude that one subject has at least 7 and the other two have at least five salience maps. A computational model shows that the primary performance-limiting factors are channel capacity for representing so many concurrently presented groups of items and working-memory capacity for so many computed centroids. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-10214162 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2023 |
publisher | National Academy of Sciences |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-102141622023-11-15 Deriving the number of salience maps an observer has from the number and quality of concurrent centroid judgments Gan, Lingyu Sun, Peng Sperling, George Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A Social Sciences [C. Koch, S. Ullman, Hum. Neurobiol.4, 219–227 (1985)] proposed a 2D topographical salience map that took feature-map outputs as its input and represented the importance “saliency” of the feature inputs at each location as a real number. The computation on the map, “winner-take-all,” was used to predict action priority. We propose that the same or a similar map is used to compute centroid judgments, the center of a cloud of diverse items. [P. Sun, V. Chu, G. Sperling, Atten. Percept. Psychophys.83, 934–955 (2021)] demonstrated that following a 250-msec exposure of a 24-dot array of 3 intermixed colors, subjects could accurately report the centroid of each dot color, thereby indicating that these subjects had at least three salience maps. Here, we use a postcue, partial-report paradigm to determine how many more salience maps subjects might have. In 11 experiments, subjects viewed 0.3-s flashes of 28 to 32 item arrays composed of M, M = 3,...,8, different features followed by a cue to mouse-click the centroid of items of just the post-cued feature. Ideal detector response analyses show that subjects utilized at least 12 to 17 stimulus items. By determining whether a subject’s performance in (M-1)-feature experiments could/could-not predict performance in M-feature experiments, we conclude that one subject has at least 7 and the other two have at least five salience maps. A computational model shows that the primary performance-limiting factors are channel capacity for representing so many concurrently presented groups of items and working-memory capacity for so many computed centroids. National Academy of Sciences 2023-05-15 2023-05-23 /pmc/articles/PMC10214162/ /pubmed/37186842 http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2301707120 Text en Copyright © 2023 the Author(s). Published by PNAS. https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/This article is distributed under Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives License 4.0 (CC BY-NC-ND) (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/) . |
spellingShingle | Social Sciences Gan, Lingyu Sun, Peng Sperling, George Deriving the number of salience maps an observer has from the number and quality of concurrent centroid judgments |
title | Deriving the number of salience maps an observer has from the number and quality of concurrent centroid judgments |
title_full | Deriving the number of salience maps an observer has from the number and quality of concurrent centroid judgments |
title_fullStr | Deriving the number of salience maps an observer has from the number and quality of concurrent centroid judgments |
title_full_unstemmed | Deriving the number of salience maps an observer has from the number and quality of concurrent centroid judgments |
title_short | Deriving the number of salience maps an observer has from the number and quality of concurrent centroid judgments |
title_sort | deriving the number of salience maps an observer has from the number and quality of concurrent centroid judgments |
topic | Social Sciences |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10214162/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/37186842 http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2301707120 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT ganlingyu derivingthenumberofsaliencemapsanobserverhasfromthenumberandqualityofconcurrentcentroidjudgments AT sunpeng derivingthenumberofsaliencemapsanobserverhasfromthenumberandqualityofconcurrentcentroidjudgments AT sperlinggeorge derivingthenumberofsaliencemapsanobserverhasfromthenumberandqualityofconcurrentcentroidjudgments |