Cargando…

Deriving the number of salience maps an observer has from the number and quality of concurrent centroid judgments

[C. Koch, S. Ullman, Hum. Neurobiol.4, 219–227 (1985)] proposed a 2D topographical salience map that took feature-map outputs as its input and represented the importance “saliency” of the feature inputs at each location as a real number. The computation on the map, “winner-take-all,” was used to pre...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Gan, Lingyu, Sun, Peng, Sperling, George
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: National Academy of Sciences 2023
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10214162/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/37186842
http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2301707120
_version_ 1785145882855866368
author Gan, Lingyu
Sun, Peng
Sperling, George
author_facet Gan, Lingyu
Sun, Peng
Sperling, George
author_sort Gan, Lingyu
collection PubMed
description [C. Koch, S. Ullman, Hum. Neurobiol.4, 219–227 (1985)] proposed a 2D topographical salience map that took feature-map outputs as its input and represented the importance “saliency” of the feature inputs at each location as a real number. The computation on the map, “winner-take-all,” was used to predict action priority. We propose that the same or a similar map is used to compute centroid judgments, the center of a cloud of diverse items. [P. Sun, V. Chu, G. Sperling, Atten. Percept. Psychophys.83, 934–955 (2021)] demonstrated that following a 250-msec exposure of a 24-dot array of 3 intermixed colors, subjects could accurately report the centroid of each dot color, thereby indicating that these subjects had at least three salience maps. Here, we use a postcue, partial-report paradigm to determine how many more salience maps subjects might have. In 11 experiments, subjects viewed 0.3-s flashes of 28 to 32 item arrays composed of M, M = 3,...,8, different features followed by a cue to mouse-click the centroid of items of just the post-cued feature. Ideal detector response analyses show that subjects utilized at least 12 to 17 stimulus items. By determining whether a subject’s performance in (M-1)-feature experiments could/could-not predict performance in M-feature experiments, we conclude that one subject has at least 7 and the other two have at least five salience maps. A computational model shows that the primary performance-limiting factors are channel capacity for representing so many concurrently presented groups of items and working-memory capacity for so many computed centroids.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-10214162
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2023
publisher National Academy of Sciences
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-102141622023-11-15 Deriving the number of salience maps an observer has from the number and quality of concurrent centroid judgments Gan, Lingyu Sun, Peng Sperling, George Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A Social Sciences [C. Koch, S. Ullman, Hum. Neurobiol.4, 219–227 (1985)] proposed a 2D topographical salience map that took feature-map outputs as its input and represented the importance “saliency” of the feature inputs at each location as a real number. The computation on the map, “winner-take-all,” was used to predict action priority. We propose that the same or a similar map is used to compute centroid judgments, the center of a cloud of diverse items. [P. Sun, V. Chu, G. Sperling, Atten. Percept. Psychophys.83, 934–955 (2021)] demonstrated that following a 250-msec exposure of a 24-dot array of 3 intermixed colors, subjects could accurately report the centroid of each dot color, thereby indicating that these subjects had at least three salience maps. Here, we use a postcue, partial-report paradigm to determine how many more salience maps subjects might have. In 11 experiments, subjects viewed 0.3-s flashes of 28 to 32 item arrays composed of M, M = 3,...,8, different features followed by a cue to mouse-click the centroid of items of just the post-cued feature. Ideal detector response analyses show that subjects utilized at least 12 to 17 stimulus items. By determining whether a subject’s performance in (M-1)-feature experiments could/could-not predict performance in M-feature experiments, we conclude that one subject has at least 7 and the other two have at least five salience maps. A computational model shows that the primary performance-limiting factors are channel capacity for representing so many concurrently presented groups of items and working-memory capacity for so many computed centroids. National Academy of Sciences 2023-05-15 2023-05-23 /pmc/articles/PMC10214162/ /pubmed/37186842 http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2301707120 Text en Copyright © 2023 the Author(s). Published by PNAS. https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/This article is distributed under Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives License 4.0 (CC BY-NC-ND) (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/) .
spellingShingle Social Sciences
Gan, Lingyu
Sun, Peng
Sperling, George
Deriving the number of salience maps an observer has from the number and quality of concurrent centroid judgments
title Deriving the number of salience maps an observer has from the number and quality of concurrent centroid judgments
title_full Deriving the number of salience maps an observer has from the number and quality of concurrent centroid judgments
title_fullStr Deriving the number of salience maps an observer has from the number and quality of concurrent centroid judgments
title_full_unstemmed Deriving the number of salience maps an observer has from the number and quality of concurrent centroid judgments
title_short Deriving the number of salience maps an observer has from the number and quality of concurrent centroid judgments
title_sort deriving the number of salience maps an observer has from the number and quality of concurrent centroid judgments
topic Social Sciences
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10214162/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/37186842
http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2301707120
work_keys_str_mv AT ganlingyu derivingthenumberofsaliencemapsanobserverhasfromthenumberandqualityofconcurrentcentroidjudgments
AT sunpeng derivingthenumberofsaliencemapsanobserverhasfromthenumberandqualityofconcurrentcentroidjudgments
AT sperlinggeorge derivingthenumberofsaliencemapsanobserverhasfromthenumberandqualityofconcurrentcentroidjudgments