Cargando…
Conflict in a word‐based approach‐avoidance task is stronger with positive words
BACKGROUND: Valence and motivational direction are linked. We approach good things and avoid bad things, and experience overriding these links as conflicting. Positive valence is more consistently linked with approach than negative valence is linked with avoidance. Therefore, avoiding positive stimu...
Autores principales: | , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
John Wiley and Sons Inc.
2023
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10275559/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/37165754 http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/brb3.3008 |
_version_ | 1785059897766838272 |
---|---|
author | Klackl, Johannes Blechert, Jens Jonas, Eva |
author_facet | Klackl, Johannes Blechert, Jens Jonas, Eva |
author_sort | Klackl, Johannes |
collection | PubMed |
description | BACKGROUND: Valence and motivational direction are linked. We approach good things and avoid bad things, and experience overriding these links as conflicting. Positive valence is more consistently linked with approach than negative valence is linked with avoidance. Therefore, avoiding positive stimuli should produce greater behavioral and neural signs of conflict than approaching negative stimuli. METHODS: In the present event‐related potential study, we tested this assumption by contrasting positive and negative conflict. We used the manikin task, in which we read positive and negative words that they needed to approach and avoid. RESULTS: Consistent with our prediction, positive conflict prolonged reaction times more than negative conflict did. A late (500–1000 ms following word onset) event‐related potential that we identified as the Conflict slow potential, was only sensitive to positive conflict. CONCLUSION: The results of this study support the notion that avoiding positive stimuli is more conflicting than approaching negative stimuli. The fact that the conflict slow potential is typically sensitive to response conflict rather than stimulus conflict suggests that the manikin task primarily requires people to override prepotent responses rather than to identify conflicting stimuli. Thus, the present findings also shed light on the psychological processes subserving conflict resolution in the manikin task. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-10275559 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2023 |
publisher | John Wiley and Sons Inc. |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-102755592023-06-17 Conflict in a word‐based approach‐avoidance task is stronger with positive words Klackl, Johannes Blechert, Jens Jonas, Eva Brain Behav Original Articles BACKGROUND: Valence and motivational direction are linked. We approach good things and avoid bad things, and experience overriding these links as conflicting. Positive valence is more consistently linked with approach than negative valence is linked with avoidance. Therefore, avoiding positive stimuli should produce greater behavioral and neural signs of conflict than approaching negative stimuli. METHODS: In the present event‐related potential study, we tested this assumption by contrasting positive and negative conflict. We used the manikin task, in which we read positive and negative words that they needed to approach and avoid. RESULTS: Consistent with our prediction, positive conflict prolonged reaction times more than negative conflict did. A late (500–1000 ms following word onset) event‐related potential that we identified as the Conflict slow potential, was only sensitive to positive conflict. CONCLUSION: The results of this study support the notion that avoiding positive stimuli is more conflicting than approaching negative stimuli. The fact that the conflict slow potential is typically sensitive to response conflict rather than stimulus conflict suggests that the manikin task primarily requires people to override prepotent responses rather than to identify conflicting stimuli. Thus, the present findings also shed light on the psychological processes subserving conflict resolution in the manikin task. John Wiley and Sons Inc. 2023-05-11 /pmc/articles/PMC10275559/ /pubmed/37165754 http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/brb3.3008 Text en © 2023 The Authors. Brain and Behavior published by Wiley Periodicals LLC. https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/This is an open access article under the terms of the http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. |
spellingShingle | Original Articles Klackl, Johannes Blechert, Jens Jonas, Eva Conflict in a word‐based approach‐avoidance task is stronger with positive words |
title | Conflict in a word‐based approach‐avoidance task is stronger with positive words |
title_full | Conflict in a word‐based approach‐avoidance task is stronger with positive words |
title_fullStr | Conflict in a word‐based approach‐avoidance task is stronger with positive words |
title_full_unstemmed | Conflict in a word‐based approach‐avoidance task is stronger with positive words |
title_short | Conflict in a word‐based approach‐avoidance task is stronger with positive words |
title_sort | conflict in a word‐based approach‐avoidance task is stronger with positive words |
topic | Original Articles |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10275559/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/37165754 http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/brb3.3008 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT klackljohannes conflictinawordbasedapproachavoidancetaskisstrongerwithpositivewords AT blechertjens conflictinawordbasedapproachavoidancetaskisstrongerwithpositivewords AT jonaseva conflictinawordbasedapproachavoidancetaskisstrongerwithpositivewords |