Cargando…
No Signs of Inclusive Fitness or Reciprocal Altruism in Advantageous Inequity Aversion
Advantageous inequity aversion (i.e., the tendency to respond negatively to unfairness that benefits oneself) usually develops in 6–8-year-olds. However, little is known about the selection pressures that might have shaped this phenomenon. Using data collected from 120 4–8-year-old Finnish children,...
Autores principales: | , , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
SAGE Publications
2023
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10303545/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/37198896 http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/14747049231173401 |
_version_ | 1785065302163193856 |
---|---|
author | Antfolk, Jan Marklund, Emmie Nylund, Irene Gunst, Annika |
author_facet | Antfolk, Jan Marklund, Emmie Nylund, Irene Gunst, Annika |
author_sort | Antfolk, Jan |
collection | PubMed |
description | Advantageous inequity aversion (i.e., the tendency to respond negatively to unfairness that benefits oneself) usually develops in 6–8-year-olds. However, little is known about the selection pressures that might have shaped this phenomenon. Using data collected from 120 4–8-year-old Finnish children, we tested two evolutionary explanations for the development of advantageous inequity aversion: reciprocal altruism (i.e., benefiting from sharing when the roles are likely reversed in the future) and inclusive fitness (i.e., benefiting from sharing with biological relatives that carry the same alleles). We first successfully replicated a previous experiment, showing that 6–8-year-olds display advantageous inequity aversion by preferring to throw away a resource rather than keep it for themselves. Here, this behavior was also displayed in 5-year-olds. Using a novel experiment, we then asked children to distribute five erasers between themselves, a sibling, a peer, and a stranger. That is, an equal distribution was only possible if throwing away one eraser. We found no support for advantageous inequity aversion being shaped by either inclusive fitness or reciprocal altruism. Future studies could investigate costly signaling and adherence to social norms to avoid negative consequences as ultimate explanations for advantageous inequity aversion. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-10303545 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2023 |
publisher | SAGE Publications |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-103035452023-08-17 No Signs of Inclusive Fitness or Reciprocal Altruism in Advantageous Inequity Aversion Antfolk, Jan Marklund, Emmie Nylund, Irene Gunst, Annika Evol Psychol Original Research Article Advantageous inequity aversion (i.e., the tendency to respond negatively to unfairness that benefits oneself) usually develops in 6–8-year-olds. However, little is known about the selection pressures that might have shaped this phenomenon. Using data collected from 120 4–8-year-old Finnish children, we tested two evolutionary explanations for the development of advantageous inequity aversion: reciprocal altruism (i.e., benefiting from sharing when the roles are likely reversed in the future) and inclusive fitness (i.e., benefiting from sharing with biological relatives that carry the same alleles). We first successfully replicated a previous experiment, showing that 6–8-year-olds display advantageous inequity aversion by preferring to throw away a resource rather than keep it for themselves. Here, this behavior was also displayed in 5-year-olds. Using a novel experiment, we then asked children to distribute five erasers between themselves, a sibling, a peer, and a stranger. That is, an equal distribution was only possible if throwing away one eraser. We found no support for advantageous inequity aversion being shaped by either inclusive fitness or reciprocal altruism. Future studies could investigate costly signaling and adherence to social norms to avoid negative consequences as ultimate explanations for advantageous inequity aversion. SAGE Publications 2023-05-17 /pmc/articles/PMC10303545/ /pubmed/37198896 http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/14747049231173401 Text en © The Author(s) 2023 https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) which permits any use, reproduction and distribution of the work without further permission provided the original work is attributed as specified on the SAGE and Open Access page (https://us.sagepub.com/en-us/nam/open-access-at-sage). |
spellingShingle | Original Research Article Antfolk, Jan Marklund, Emmie Nylund, Irene Gunst, Annika No Signs of Inclusive Fitness or Reciprocal Altruism in Advantageous Inequity Aversion |
title | No Signs of Inclusive Fitness or Reciprocal Altruism in Advantageous Inequity
Aversion |
title_full | No Signs of Inclusive Fitness or Reciprocal Altruism in Advantageous Inequity
Aversion |
title_fullStr | No Signs of Inclusive Fitness or Reciprocal Altruism in Advantageous Inequity
Aversion |
title_full_unstemmed | No Signs of Inclusive Fitness or Reciprocal Altruism in Advantageous Inequity
Aversion |
title_short | No Signs of Inclusive Fitness or Reciprocal Altruism in Advantageous Inequity
Aversion |
title_sort | no signs of inclusive fitness or reciprocal altruism in advantageous inequity
aversion |
topic | Original Research Article |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10303545/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/37198896 http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/14747049231173401 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT antfolkjan nosignsofinclusivefitnessorreciprocalaltruisminadvantageousinequityaversion AT marklundemmie nosignsofinclusivefitnessorreciprocalaltruisminadvantageousinequityaversion AT nylundirene nosignsofinclusivefitnessorreciprocalaltruisminadvantageousinequityaversion AT gunstannika nosignsofinclusivefitnessorreciprocalaltruisminadvantageousinequityaversion |