Cargando…

No Signs of Inclusive Fitness or Reciprocal Altruism in Advantageous Inequity Aversion

Advantageous inequity aversion (i.e., the tendency to respond negatively to unfairness that benefits oneself) usually develops in 6–8-year-olds. However, little is known about the selection pressures that might have shaped this phenomenon. Using data collected from 120 4–8-year-old Finnish children,...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Antfolk, Jan, Marklund, Emmie, Nylund, Irene, Gunst, Annika
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: SAGE Publications 2023
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10303545/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/37198896
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/14747049231173401
_version_ 1785065302163193856
author Antfolk, Jan
Marklund, Emmie
Nylund, Irene
Gunst, Annika
author_facet Antfolk, Jan
Marklund, Emmie
Nylund, Irene
Gunst, Annika
author_sort Antfolk, Jan
collection PubMed
description Advantageous inequity aversion (i.e., the tendency to respond negatively to unfairness that benefits oneself) usually develops in 6–8-year-olds. However, little is known about the selection pressures that might have shaped this phenomenon. Using data collected from 120 4–8-year-old Finnish children, we tested two evolutionary explanations for the development of advantageous inequity aversion: reciprocal altruism (i.e., benefiting from sharing when the roles are likely reversed in the future) and inclusive fitness (i.e., benefiting from sharing with biological relatives that carry the same alleles). We first successfully replicated a previous experiment, showing that 6–8-year-olds display advantageous inequity aversion by preferring to throw away a resource rather than keep it for themselves. Here, this behavior was also displayed in 5-year-olds. Using a novel experiment, we then asked children to distribute five erasers between themselves, a sibling, a peer, and a stranger. That is, an equal distribution was only possible if throwing away one eraser. We found no support for advantageous inequity aversion being shaped by either inclusive fitness or reciprocal altruism. Future studies could investigate costly signaling and adherence to social norms to avoid negative consequences as ultimate explanations for advantageous inequity aversion.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-10303545
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2023
publisher SAGE Publications
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-103035452023-08-17 No Signs of Inclusive Fitness or Reciprocal Altruism in Advantageous Inequity Aversion Antfolk, Jan Marklund, Emmie Nylund, Irene Gunst, Annika Evol Psychol Original Research Article Advantageous inequity aversion (i.e., the tendency to respond negatively to unfairness that benefits oneself) usually develops in 6–8-year-olds. However, little is known about the selection pressures that might have shaped this phenomenon. Using data collected from 120 4–8-year-old Finnish children, we tested two evolutionary explanations for the development of advantageous inequity aversion: reciprocal altruism (i.e., benefiting from sharing when the roles are likely reversed in the future) and inclusive fitness (i.e., benefiting from sharing with biological relatives that carry the same alleles). We first successfully replicated a previous experiment, showing that 6–8-year-olds display advantageous inequity aversion by preferring to throw away a resource rather than keep it for themselves. Here, this behavior was also displayed in 5-year-olds. Using a novel experiment, we then asked children to distribute five erasers between themselves, a sibling, a peer, and a stranger. That is, an equal distribution was only possible if throwing away one eraser. We found no support for advantageous inequity aversion being shaped by either inclusive fitness or reciprocal altruism. Future studies could investigate costly signaling and adherence to social norms to avoid negative consequences as ultimate explanations for advantageous inequity aversion. SAGE Publications 2023-05-17 /pmc/articles/PMC10303545/ /pubmed/37198896 http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/14747049231173401 Text en © The Author(s) 2023 https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) which permits any use, reproduction and distribution of the work without further permission provided the original work is attributed as specified on the SAGE and Open Access page (https://us.sagepub.com/en-us/nam/open-access-at-sage).
spellingShingle Original Research Article
Antfolk, Jan
Marklund, Emmie
Nylund, Irene
Gunst, Annika
No Signs of Inclusive Fitness or Reciprocal Altruism in Advantageous Inequity Aversion
title No Signs of Inclusive Fitness or Reciprocal Altruism in Advantageous Inequity Aversion
title_full No Signs of Inclusive Fitness or Reciprocal Altruism in Advantageous Inequity Aversion
title_fullStr No Signs of Inclusive Fitness or Reciprocal Altruism in Advantageous Inequity Aversion
title_full_unstemmed No Signs of Inclusive Fitness or Reciprocal Altruism in Advantageous Inequity Aversion
title_short No Signs of Inclusive Fitness or Reciprocal Altruism in Advantageous Inequity Aversion
title_sort no signs of inclusive fitness or reciprocal altruism in advantageous inequity aversion
topic Original Research Article
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10303545/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/37198896
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/14747049231173401
work_keys_str_mv AT antfolkjan nosignsofinclusivefitnessorreciprocalaltruisminadvantageousinequityaversion
AT marklundemmie nosignsofinclusivefitnessorreciprocalaltruisminadvantageousinequityaversion
AT nylundirene nosignsofinclusivefitnessorreciprocalaltruisminadvantageousinequityaversion
AT gunstannika nosignsofinclusivefitnessorreciprocalaltruisminadvantageousinequityaversion