Cargando…
Kin term mimicry hypothesis
Adaptive mimicry in animals is a well-known phenomenon. Here, we propose that a similarly adaptive strategy in humans is using kin terms for people who are not closely genetically related. Irrespective of the initiator attributing a kin term to a non-kin, we call this kin term mimicry (KTM). The eme...
Autores principales: | , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
2023
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10423118/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/37277580 http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12064-023-00393-1 |
_version_ | 1785089375663554560 |
---|---|
author | Pawłowski, Bogusław Chmielińska, Anna |
author_facet | Pawłowski, Bogusław Chmielińska, Anna |
author_sort | Pawłowski, Bogusław |
collection | PubMed |
description | Adaptive mimicry in animals is a well-known phenomenon. Here, we propose that a similarly adaptive strategy in humans is using kin terms for people who are not closely genetically related. Irrespective of the initiator attributing a kin term to a non-kin, we call this kin term mimicry (KTM). The emergence of human sociality and language allowed not only easy kin recognition, but also led to strong positive emotions related to such kin names as “mother,” “father,” “brother,” “sister,” “aunt” or “uncle.” Although the phenomenon of using kin terms of genetically unrelated people is well known in the social sciences, here we discuss it in the light of evolution. We notice this is an evolutionary adaptive cooperation strategy, which allows us to predict in which ecological or social circumstances it will be more prevalent. We postulate specific testable factors that affect the prevalence of kin mimicry. We also discuss who is more likely to be an initiator of calling non-kin a fictive kin, and who benefits from such behavior. The KTM hypothesis postulates that an individual or social group initiating or bestowing kin terms usually receives more benefits (economic and/or psychological support) from such mimicry. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-10423118 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2023 |
publisher | Springer Berlin Heidelberg |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-104231182023-08-14 Kin term mimicry hypothesis Pawłowski, Bogusław Chmielińska, Anna Theory Biosci Original Article Adaptive mimicry in animals is a well-known phenomenon. Here, we propose that a similarly adaptive strategy in humans is using kin terms for people who are not closely genetically related. Irrespective of the initiator attributing a kin term to a non-kin, we call this kin term mimicry (KTM). The emergence of human sociality and language allowed not only easy kin recognition, but also led to strong positive emotions related to such kin names as “mother,” “father,” “brother,” “sister,” “aunt” or “uncle.” Although the phenomenon of using kin terms of genetically unrelated people is well known in the social sciences, here we discuss it in the light of evolution. We notice this is an evolutionary adaptive cooperation strategy, which allows us to predict in which ecological or social circumstances it will be more prevalent. We postulate specific testable factors that affect the prevalence of kin mimicry. We also discuss who is more likely to be an initiator of calling non-kin a fictive kin, and who benefits from such behavior. The KTM hypothesis postulates that an individual or social group initiating or bestowing kin terms usually receives more benefits (economic and/or psychological support) from such mimicry. Springer Berlin Heidelberg 2023-06-05 2023 /pmc/articles/PMC10423118/ /pubmed/37277580 http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12064-023-00393-1 Text en © The Author(s) 2023 https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) . |
spellingShingle | Original Article Pawłowski, Bogusław Chmielińska, Anna Kin term mimicry hypothesis |
title | Kin term mimicry hypothesis |
title_full | Kin term mimicry hypothesis |
title_fullStr | Kin term mimicry hypothesis |
title_full_unstemmed | Kin term mimicry hypothesis |
title_short | Kin term mimicry hypothesis |
title_sort | kin term mimicry hypothesis |
topic | Original Article |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10423118/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/37277580 http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12064-023-00393-1 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT pawłowskibogusław kintermmimicryhypothesis AT chmielinskaanna kintermmimicryhypothesis |