Cargando…
Sex-biased sound symbolism in French first names
SUMMARY: Low- and high-frequency vowels in the stressed syllable of French first names may respectively project impressions of largeness/masculinity and smallness/femininity. ABSTRACT: Given that first names can have a lifelong impact on the bearer, parents should choose a name based on the impressi...
Autores principales: | , , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
Cambridge University Press
2019
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10427910/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/37588406 http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/ehs.2019.7 |
_version_ | 1785090348895174656 |
---|---|
author | Suire, Alexandre Bossoms Mesa, Alba Raymond, Michel Barkat-Defradas, Melissa |
author_facet | Suire, Alexandre Bossoms Mesa, Alba Raymond, Michel Barkat-Defradas, Melissa |
author_sort | Suire, Alexandre |
collection | PubMed |
description | SUMMARY: Low- and high-frequency vowels in the stressed syllable of French first names may respectively project impressions of largeness/masculinity and smallness/femininity. ABSTRACT: Given that first names can have a lifelong impact on the bearer, parents should choose a name based on the impressions they want their offspring to evoke in other people. This name-to-mental-image association can be mediated through sound symbolism: a natural link between the sounds and meaning of a word. From an evolutionary perspective, parents should pick names which sounds convey traits advantageous in human sexual selection: largeness and masculinity for males through lower-frequency sounds as opposed to smallness and femininity for females through higher-frequency sounds. Using a database of French first names from 1900 to 2009, we observed a sex-biased sound symbolism pattern in the last syllable, which is the perceptually prominent one in French. Male names were more likely to include lower-frequency vowels (e.g. /o/, /ã/) and female names higher-frequency vowels (e.g. /i/, /e/). Unexpected patterns in consonants were observed in masculine names with higher-frequency sounds (e.g. /s/, /ʃ/) in the last syllable and lower-frequency sounds (e.g. /b/, /g/) in the first syllable. However, little variance was explained and the modest size effect suggests that cultural traits influence these sex differences. Lastly, exploratory analyses revealed a phonetic masculinization in women's first names that has increased since the 1960s. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-10427910 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2019 |
publisher | Cambridge University Press |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-104279102023-08-16 Sex-biased sound symbolism in French first names Suire, Alexandre Bossoms Mesa, Alba Raymond, Michel Barkat-Defradas, Melissa Evol Hum Sci Research Article SUMMARY: Low- and high-frequency vowels in the stressed syllable of French first names may respectively project impressions of largeness/masculinity and smallness/femininity. ABSTRACT: Given that first names can have a lifelong impact on the bearer, parents should choose a name based on the impressions they want their offspring to evoke in other people. This name-to-mental-image association can be mediated through sound symbolism: a natural link between the sounds and meaning of a word. From an evolutionary perspective, parents should pick names which sounds convey traits advantageous in human sexual selection: largeness and masculinity for males through lower-frequency sounds as opposed to smallness and femininity for females through higher-frequency sounds. Using a database of French first names from 1900 to 2009, we observed a sex-biased sound symbolism pattern in the last syllable, which is the perceptually prominent one in French. Male names were more likely to include lower-frequency vowels (e.g. /o/, /ã/) and female names higher-frequency vowels (e.g. /i/, /e/). Unexpected patterns in consonants were observed in masculine names with higher-frequency sounds (e.g. /s/, /ʃ/) in the last syllable and lower-frequency sounds (e.g. /b/, /g/) in the first syllable. However, little variance was explained and the modest size effect suggests that cultural traits influence these sex differences. Lastly, exploratory analyses revealed a phonetic masculinization in women's first names that has increased since the 1960s. Cambridge University Press 2019-07-12 /pmc/articles/PMC10427910/ /pubmed/37588406 http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/ehs.2019.7 Text en © The Author(s) 2019 https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) ), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. |
spellingShingle | Research Article Suire, Alexandre Bossoms Mesa, Alba Raymond, Michel Barkat-Defradas, Melissa Sex-biased sound symbolism in French first names |
title | Sex-biased sound symbolism in French first names |
title_full | Sex-biased sound symbolism in French first names |
title_fullStr | Sex-biased sound symbolism in French first names |
title_full_unstemmed | Sex-biased sound symbolism in French first names |
title_short | Sex-biased sound symbolism in French first names |
title_sort | sex-biased sound symbolism in french first names |
topic | Research Article |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10427910/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/37588406 http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/ehs.2019.7 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT suirealexandre sexbiasedsoundsymbolisminfrenchfirstnames AT bossomsmesaalba sexbiasedsoundsymbolisminfrenchfirstnames AT raymondmichel sexbiasedsoundsymbolisminfrenchfirstnames AT barkatdefradasmelissa sexbiasedsoundsymbolisminfrenchfirstnames |