Cargando…
The color communication game
There is clear diversity among speakers of a typical language in how colors are named. What is the impact of this diversity on the people’s ability to communicate about color? Is there a gap between a person’s general understanding of the color terms in their native language and how they understand...
Autores principales: | , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
Nature Publishing Group UK
2023
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10520057/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/37749107 http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41598-023-42834-3 |
_version_ | 1785109829311791104 |
---|---|
author | Brown, Angela M. Lindsey, Delwin T. |
author_facet | Brown, Angela M. Lindsey, Delwin T. |
author_sort | Brown, Angela M. |
collection | PubMed |
description | There is clear diversity among speakers of a typical language in how colors are named. What is the impact of this diversity on the people’s ability to communicate about color? Is there a gap between a person’s general understanding of the color terms in their native language and how they understand a particular term that denotes a particular color sample? Seventy English-speaking dyads and 63 Somali-speaking dyads played the Color Communication Game, where the “sender” in each dyad named 30 color samples as they would in any color-naming study, then the “receiver” chose the sample they thought the sender intended to communicate. English speakers played again, under instructions to intentionally communicate color sample identity. Direct comparison of senders’ samples and receivers’ choices revealed categorical understanding of colors without considering color naming data. Although Somali-speaking senders provided fewer color terms, interpersonal Mutual Information (MI) calculated from color naming data was similarly below optimal for both groups, and English-speaking dyads’ MI did not improve with experience. Both groups revealed superior understanding of color terms because receivers showed better exactly-correct selection performance than was predicted by simulation from their senders’ color-naming data. This study highlights limitations on information-theoretic analyses of color naming data. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-10520057 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2023 |
publisher | Nature Publishing Group UK |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-105200572023-09-27 The color communication game Brown, Angela M. Lindsey, Delwin T. Sci Rep Article There is clear diversity among speakers of a typical language in how colors are named. What is the impact of this diversity on the people’s ability to communicate about color? Is there a gap between a person’s general understanding of the color terms in their native language and how they understand a particular term that denotes a particular color sample? Seventy English-speaking dyads and 63 Somali-speaking dyads played the Color Communication Game, where the “sender” in each dyad named 30 color samples as they would in any color-naming study, then the “receiver” chose the sample they thought the sender intended to communicate. English speakers played again, under instructions to intentionally communicate color sample identity. Direct comparison of senders’ samples and receivers’ choices revealed categorical understanding of colors without considering color naming data. Although Somali-speaking senders provided fewer color terms, interpersonal Mutual Information (MI) calculated from color naming data was similarly below optimal for both groups, and English-speaking dyads’ MI did not improve with experience. Both groups revealed superior understanding of color terms because receivers showed better exactly-correct selection performance than was predicted by simulation from their senders’ color-naming data. This study highlights limitations on information-theoretic analyses of color naming data. Nature Publishing Group UK 2023-09-25 /pmc/articles/PMC10520057/ /pubmed/37749107 http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41598-023-42834-3 Text en © The Author(s) 2023 https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) . |
spellingShingle | Article Brown, Angela M. Lindsey, Delwin T. The color communication game |
title | The color communication game |
title_full | The color communication game |
title_fullStr | The color communication game |
title_full_unstemmed | The color communication game |
title_short | The color communication game |
title_sort | color communication game |
topic | Article |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10520057/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/37749107 http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41598-023-42834-3 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT brownangelam thecolorcommunicationgame AT lindseydelwint thecolorcommunicationgame AT brownangelam colorcommunicationgame AT lindseydelwint colorcommunicationgame |