Cargando…

Feeling sleepy? stop driving—awareness of fall asleep crashes

STUDY OBJECTIVES: To examine whether drivers are aware of sleepiness and associated symptoms, and how subjective reports predict driving impairment and physiological drowsiness. METHODS: Sixteen shift workers (19–65 years; 9 women) drove an instrumented vehicle for 2 hours on a closed-loop track aft...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Anderson, Clare, Cai, Anna W T, Lee, Michael L, Horrey, William J, Liang, Yulan, O’Brien, Conor S, Czeisler, Charles A, Howard, Mark E
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: Oxford University Press 2023
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10636256/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/37158173
http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/sleep/zsad136
_version_ 1785133172711751680
author Anderson, Clare
Cai, Anna W T
Lee, Michael L
Horrey, William J
Liang, Yulan
O’Brien, Conor S
Czeisler, Charles A
Howard, Mark E
author_facet Anderson, Clare
Cai, Anna W T
Lee, Michael L
Horrey, William J
Liang, Yulan
O’Brien, Conor S
Czeisler, Charles A
Howard, Mark E
author_sort Anderson, Clare
collection PubMed
description STUDY OBJECTIVES: To examine whether drivers are aware of sleepiness and associated symptoms, and how subjective reports predict driving impairment and physiological drowsiness. METHODS: Sixteen shift workers (19–65 years; 9 women) drove an instrumented vehicle for 2 hours on a closed-loop track after a night of sleep and a night of work. Subjective sleepiness/symptoms were rated every 15 minutes. Severe and moderate driving impairment was defined by emergency brake maneuvers and lane deviations, respectively. Physiological drowsiness was defined by eye closures (Johns drowsiness scores) and EEG-based microsleep events. RESULTS: All subjective ratings increased post night-shift (p < 0.001). No severe drive events occurred without noticeable symptoms beforehand. All subjective sleepiness ratings, and specific symptoms, predicted a severe (emergency brake) driving event occurring in the next 15 minutes (OR: 1.76–2.4, AUC > 0.81, p < 0.009), except “head dropping down”. Karolinska Sleepiness Scale (KSS), ocular symptoms, difficulty keeping to center of the road, and nodding off to sleep, were associated with a lane deviation in the next 15 minutes (OR: 1.17–1.24, p<0.029), although accuracy was only “fair” (AUC 0.59–0.65). All sleepiness ratings predicted severe ocular-based drowsiness (OR: 1.30–2.81, p < 0.001), with very good-to-excellent accuracy (AUC > 0.8), while moderate ocular-based drowsiness was predicted with fair-to-good accuracy (AUC > 0.62). KSS, likelihood of falling asleep, ocular symptoms, and “nodding off” predicted microsleep events, with fair-to-good accuracy (AUC 0.65–0.73). CONCLUSIONS: Drivers are aware of sleepiness, and many self-reported sleepiness symptoms predicted subsequent driving impairment/physiological drowsiness. Drivers should self-assess a wide range of sleepiness symptoms and stop driving when these occur to reduce the escalating risk of road crashes due to drowsiness.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-10636256
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2023
publisher Oxford University Press
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-106362562023-11-11 Feeling sleepy? stop driving—awareness of fall asleep crashes Anderson, Clare Cai, Anna W T Lee, Michael L Horrey, William J Liang, Yulan O’Brien, Conor S Czeisler, Charles A Howard, Mark E Sleep Cognitve, Affective and Behavioral Neroscence of Sleep STUDY OBJECTIVES: To examine whether drivers are aware of sleepiness and associated symptoms, and how subjective reports predict driving impairment and physiological drowsiness. METHODS: Sixteen shift workers (19–65 years; 9 women) drove an instrumented vehicle for 2 hours on a closed-loop track after a night of sleep and a night of work. Subjective sleepiness/symptoms were rated every 15 minutes. Severe and moderate driving impairment was defined by emergency brake maneuvers and lane deviations, respectively. Physiological drowsiness was defined by eye closures (Johns drowsiness scores) and EEG-based microsleep events. RESULTS: All subjective ratings increased post night-shift (p < 0.001). No severe drive events occurred without noticeable symptoms beforehand. All subjective sleepiness ratings, and specific symptoms, predicted a severe (emergency brake) driving event occurring in the next 15 minutes (OR: 1.76–2.4, AUC > 0.81, p < 0.009), except “head dropping down”. Karolinska Sleepiness Scale (KSS), ocular symptoms, difficulty keeping to center of the road, and nodding off to sleep, were associated with a lane deviation in the next 15 minutes (OR: 1.17–1.24, p<0.029), although accuracy was only “fair” (AUC 0.59–0.65). All sleepiness ratings predicted severe ocular-based drowsiness (OR: 1.30–2.81, p < 0.001), with very good-to-excellent accuracy (AUC > 0.8), while moderate ocular-based drowsiness was predicted with fair-to-good accuracy (AUC > 0.62). KSS, likelihood of falling asleep, ocular symptoms, and “nodding off” predicted microsleep events, with fair-to-good accuracy (AUC 0.65–0.73). CONCLUSIONS: Drivers are aware of sleepiness, and many self-reported sleepiness symptoms predicted subsequent driving impairment/physiological drowsiness. Drivers should self-assess a wide range of sleepiness symptoms and stop driving when these occur to reduce the escalating risk of road crashes due to drowsiness. Oxford University Press 2023-05-09 /pmc/articles/PMC10636256/ /pubmed/37158173 http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/sleep/zsad136 Text en © The Author(s) 2023. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of Sleep Research Society. https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. For commercial re-use, please contact journals.permissions@oup.com
spellingShingle Cognitve, Affective and Behavioral Neroscence of Sleep
Anderson, Clare
Cai, Anna W T
Lee, Michael L
Horrey, William J
Liang, Yulan
O’Brien, Conor S
Czeisler, Charles A
Howard, Mark E
Feeling sleepy? stop driving—awareness of fall asleep crashes
title Feeling sleepy? stop driving—awareness of fall asleep crashes
title_full Feeling sleepy? stop driving—awareness of fall asleep crashes
title_fullStr Feeling sleepy? stop driving—awareness of fall asleep crashes
title_full_unstemmed Feeling sleepy? stop driving—awareness of fall asleep crashes
title_short Feeling sleepy? stop driving—awareness of fall asleep crashes
title_sort feeling sleepy? stop driving—awareness of fall asleep crashes
topic Cognitve, Affective and Behavioral Neroscence of Sleep
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10636256/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/37158173
http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/sleep/zsad136
work_keys_str_mv AT andersonclare feelingsleepystopdrivingawarenessoffallasleepcrashes
AT caiannawt feelingsleepystopdrivingawarenessoffallasleepcrashes
AT leemichaell feelingsleepystopdrivingawarenessoffallasleepcrashes
AT horreywilliamj feelingsleepystopdrivingawarenessoffallasleepcrashes
AT liangyulan feelingsleepystopdrivingawarenessoffallasleepcrashes
AT obrienconors feelingsleepystopdrivingawarenessoffallasleepcrashes
AT czeislercharlesa feelingsleepystopdrivingawarenessoffallasleepcrashes
AT howardmarke feelingsleepystopdrivingawarenessoffallasleepcrashes