Cargando…

False Memories for Ending of Events

Memories are not perfect recordings of the past and can be subject to systematic biases. Memory distortions are often caused by our experience of what typically happens in a given situation. However, it is unclear whether memory for events is biased by the knowledge that events usually have a predic...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Raykov, Petar P., Varga, Dominika, Bird, Chris M.
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: American Psychological Association 2023
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10694998/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/37650821
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xge0001462
_version_ 1785153498311032832
author Raykov, Petar P.
Varga, Dominika
Bird, Chris M.
author_facet Raykov, Petar P.
Varga, Dominika
Bird, Chris M.
author_sort Raykov, Petar P.
collection PubMed
description Memories are not perfect recordings of the past and can be subject to systematic biases. Memory distortions are often caused by our experience of what typically happens in a given situation. However, it is unclear whether memory for events is biased by the knowledge that events usually have a predictable structure (a beginning, middle, and an end). Using video clips of everyday situations, we tested how interrupting events at unexpected time points affects memory of how those events ended. In four free recall experiments (1, 2, 4, and 5), we found that interrupting clips just before a salient piece of action was completed, resulted in the false recall of details about how the clip might have ended. We refer to this as “event extension.” On the other hand, interrupting clips just after one scene had ended and a new scene started, resulted in omissions of details about the true ending of the clip (Experiments 4 and 5). We found that these effects were present, albeit attenuated, when testing memory shortly after watching the video clips compared to a week later (Experiments 5a and 5b). The event extension effect was not present when memory was tested with a recognition paradigm (Experiment 3). Overall, we conclude that when people watch videos that violate their expectations of typical event structure, they show a bias to later recall the videos as if they had ended at a predictable event boundary, exhibiting event extension or the omission of details depending on where the original video was interrupted.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-10694998
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2023
publisher American Psychological Association
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-106949982023-12-05 False Memories for Ending of Events Raykov, Petar P. Varga, Dominika Bird, Chris M. J Exp Psychol Gen Articles Memories are not perfect recordings of the past and can be subject to systematic biases. Memory distortions are often caused by our experience of what typically happens in a given situation. However, it is unclear whether memory for events is biased by the knowledge that events usually have a predictable structure (a beginning, middle, and an end). Using video clips of everyday situations, we tested how interrupting events at unexpected time points affects memory of how those events ended. In four free recall experiments (1, 2, 4, and 5), we found that interrupting clips just before a salient piece of action was completed, resulted in the false recall of details about how the clip might have ended. We refer to this as “event extension.” On the other hand, interrupting clips just after one scene had ended and a new scene started, resulted in omissions of details about the true ending of the clip (Experiments 4 and 5). We found that these effects were present, albeit attenuated, when testing memory shortly after watching the video clips compared to a week later (Experiments 5a and 5b). The event extension effect was not present when memory was tested with a recognition paradigm (Experiment 3). Overall, we conclude that when people watch videos that violate their expectations of typical event structure, they show a bias to later recall the videos as if they had ended at a predictable event boundary, exhibiting event extension or the omission of details depending on where the original video was interrupted. American Psychological Association 2023-08-31 2023-12 /pmc/articles/PMC10694998/ /pubmed/37650821 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xge0001462 Text en © 2023 The Author(s) https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/This article has been published under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Copyright for this article is retained by the authors. Authors grant the American Psychological Association the exclusive right to publish the article and identify itself as the original publisher.
spellingShingle Articles
Raykov, Petar P.
Varga, Dominika
Bird, Chris M.
False Memories for Ending of Events
title False Memories for Ending of Events
title_full False Memories for Ending of Events
title_fullStr False Memories for Ending of Events
title_full_unstemmed False Memories for Ending of Events
title_short False Memories for Ending of Events
title_sort false memories for ending of events
topic Articles
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10694998/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/37650821
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xge0001462
work_keys_str_mv AT raykovpetarp falsememoriesforendingofevents
AT vargadominika falsememoriesforendingofevents
AT birdchrism falsememoriesforendingofevents