Cargando…
THE VISUAL FUNCTIONS OF THE COMPLETE COLORBLIND
1. The visual functions of a completely colorblind individual are compared with those of the normal. The sensibility distribution in the spectrum has a maximum at 520 mµ at all brightnesses and thus corresponds to rod vision alone. This is confirmed by studies of dark adaptation which show final thr...
Autores principales: | , , , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
The Rockefeller University Press
1948
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2147123/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18870866 |
_version_ | 1782144365716570112 |
---|---|
author | Hecht, Selig Shlaer, Simon Smith, Emil L. Haig, Charles Peskin, James C. |
author_facet | Hecht, Selig Shlaer, Simon Smith, Emil L. Haig, Charles Peskin, James C. |
author_sort | Hecht, Selig |
collection | PubMed |
description | 1. The visual functions of a completely colorblind individual are compared with those of the normal. The sensibility distribution in the spectrum has a maximum at 520 mµ at all brightnesses and thus corresponds to rod vision alone. This is confirmed by studies of dark adaptation which show final thresholds like those usually found for rod vision. Dark adaptation, measured both centrally and peripherally in the retina, is a single continuous function, and regardless of the brightness of the preceding light adaptation, is of the rapid type only, such as that found for the normal following low light adaptation. Visual acuity also shows a single continuous function like that for rod vision. 2. Both critical fusion frequency and intensity discrimination show two sections, one at low and the other at high intensities with a sharp transition from one to the other. Intensity discrimination is as good as for the normal eye, and covers much the same range. The maximal critical fusion frequency is only about 20 cycles per second as compared to 55 cycles for the normal. 3. The two sections shown by the colorblind eye for intensity discrimination and fusion frequency possess the spectral sensitivity of rod vision since the relative positions on the intensity scale are not influenced by using different parts of the spectrum. |
format | Text |
id | pubmed-2147123 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 1948 |
publisher | The Rockefeller University Press |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-21471232008-04-23 THE VISUAL FUNCTIONS OF THE COMPLETE COLORBLIND Hecht, Selig Shlaer, Simon Smith, Emil L. Haig, Charles Peskin, James C. J Gen Physiol Article 1. The visual functions of a completely colorblind individual are compared with those of the normal. The sensibility distribution in the spectrum has a maximum at 520 mµ at all brightnesses and thus corresponds to rod vision alone. This is confirmed by studies of dark adaptation which show final thresholds like those usually found for rod vision. Dark adaptation, measured both centrally and peripherally in the retina, is a single continuous function, and regardless of the brightness of the preceding light adaptation, is of the rapid type only, such as that found for the normal following low light adaptation. Visual acuity also shows a single continuous function like that for rod vision. 2. Both critical fusion frequency and intensity discrimination show two sections, one at low and the other at high intensities with a sharp transition from one to the other. Intensity discrimination is as good as for the normal eye, and covers much the same range. The maximal critical fusion frequency is only about 20 cycles per second as compared to 55 cycles for the normal. 3. The two sections shown by the colorblind eye for intensity discrimination and fusion frequency possess the spectral sensitivity of rod vision since the relative positions on the intensity scale are not influenced by using different parts of the spectrum. The Rockefeller University Press 1948-07-20 /pmc/articles/PMC2147123/ /pubmed/18870866 Text en Copyright © Copyright, 1948, by The Rockefeller Institute for Medical Research This article is distributed under the terms of an Attribution–Noncommercial–Share Alike–No Mirror Sites license for the first six months after the publication date (see http://www.rupress.org/terms). After six months it is available under a Creative Commons License (Attribution–Noncommercial–Share Alike 4.0 Unported license, as described at http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/). |
spellingShingle | Article Hecht, Selig Shlaer, Simon Smith, Emil L. Haig, Charles Peskin, James C. THE VISUAL FUNCTIONS OF THE COMPLETE COLORBLIND |
title | THE VISUAL FUNCTIONS OF THE COMPLETE COLORBLIND |
title_full | THE VISUAL FUNCTIONS OF THE COMPLETE COLORBLIND |
title_fullStr | THE VISUAL FUNCTIONS OF THE COMPLETE COLORBLIND |
title_full_unstemmed | THE VISUAL FUNCTIONS OF THE COMPLETE COLORBLIND |
title_short | THE VISUAL FUNCTIONS OF THE COMPLETE COLORBLIND |
title_sort | visual functions of the complete colorblind |
topic | Article |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2147123/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18870866 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT hechtselig thevisualfunctionsofthecompletecolorblind AT shlaersimon thevisualfunctionsofthecompletecolorblind AT smithemill thevisualfunctionsofthecompletecolorblind AT haigcharles thevisualfunctionsofthecompletecolorblind AT peskinjamesc thevisualfunctionsofthecompletecolorblind AT hechtselig visualfunctionsofthecompletecolorblind AT shlaersimon visualfunctionsofthecompletecolorblind AT smithemill visualfunctionsofthecompletecolorblind AT haigcharles visualfunctionsofthecompletecolorblind AT peskinjamesc visualfunctionsofthecompletecolorblind |