Cargando…

THEORY AND MEASUREMENT OF VISUAL MECHANISMS : X. MODIFICATIONS OF THE FLICKER RESPONSE CONTOUR, AND THE SIGNIFICANCE OF THE AVIAN PECTEN

1. When there is projected on the retina (man, monocularly) the shadow of a grid which forms a visual field in several distinct pieces (not including the fovea in the present tests), the ordinary properties of the flicker recognition contour (F vs. log I) as a function of the light-time cycle fracti...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Crozier, W. J., Wolf, Ernst
Formato: Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: The Rockefeller University Press 1944
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2238017/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19873387
_version_ 1782150442555277312
author Crozier, W. J.
Wolf, Ernst
author_facet Crozier, W. J.
Wolf, Ernst
author_sort Crozier, W. J.
collection PubMed
description 1. When there is projected on the retina (man, monocularly) the shadow of a grid which forms a visual field in several distinct pieces (not including the fovea in the present tests), the ordinary properties of the flicker recognition contour (F vs. log I) as a function of the light-time cycle fraction (t(L)) can be markedly disturbed. In the present experiments flicker was produced by the rotation of a cylinder with opaque vertical stripes. In the absence of such a grid shadow the "cone" segments of the contours form a set in which F(max.) and the abscissa of inflection are opposite but rectilinear functions of t(L), while the third parameter of the probability integral (σ'(log I)) remains constant. This is the case also with diverse other animals tested. In the data with the grid, however, analysis shows that even for low values of t(L) (up to 0.50) there occurs an enhancement of the production of elements of neural effect, so that F(max.) rises rather than falls as ordinarily with increase of t(L), although σ'(log I) stays constant and hence the total number of acting units is presumed not to change. This constitutes valid evidence for neural integration of effects due to the illumination of separated retinal patches. Beginning at t(L) = 0.75, and at 0.90, the slope of the "cone" curve is sharply increased, and the maximum F is far above its position in the absence of the grid. The decrease of σ'(log I) (the slope constant) signifies, in terms of other information, an increase in the number of acting cone units. The abscissa of inflection is also much lowered, relatively, whereas without the grid it increases as t(L) is made larger. These effects correspond subjectively to the fact that at the end-point flicker is most pronounced, on the "cone" curve, along the edges of the grid shadow where contrast is particularly evident with the longer light-times. The "rod" portion of the F - log I contour is not specifically affected by the presence of the grid shadow. Its form is obtainable at t(L) = 0.90 free from the influence of summating "cone" contributions, because then almost no overlapping occurs. Analysis shows that when overlapping does occur a certain number of rod units are inhibited by concurrent cone excitation, and that the mean contribution of elements of neural action from each of the non-inhibited units is also reduced to an extent depending on the degree of overlap. The isolated "rod" curve at t(L) = 0.90 is quite accurately in the form of a probability integral. The data thus give a new experimental proof of the occurrence of two distinct but interlocking populations of visual effects, and experimentally justify the analytical procedures which have been used to separate them. 2. The changing form of the F - log I contour as a function of t(L), produced in man when the illuminated field is divided into parts by a shadow pattern, is normally found with the bird Taeniopygia castenotis (Gould), the zebra finch. The retina has elements of one general structural type (cones), and the F - log I contour is a simplex symmetrical probability integral. The eye of this bird has a large, complex, and darkly pigmented pecten, which casts a foliated shadow on the retina. The change in form of the F - log I curve occurs with t(L) above 0,50, and at t(L) = 0.90 is quite extreme. It is more pronounced than the one that is secured in the human data with the particular grid we have used, but there is no doubt that it could be mimicked completely by the use of other grids. The increase of flicker acuity due to the pecten shadow is considerable, when the dark spaces are brief relative to the light. The evidence thus confirms the suggestion (Menner) drawn from comparative natural history that the visual significance of the avian pecten might be to increase the sensory effect of small moving images. It is theoretically important that (as in the human experiment) this may be brought about by an actual decrease of effective retinal area illuminated. It is also significant theoretically that despite the presence of shadows of pecten or of grid, and of the sensory influences thus introduced, the probability integral formulation remains effective.
format Text
id pubmed-2238017
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 1944
publisher The Rockefeller University Press
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-22380172008-04-23 THEORY AND MEASUREMENT OF VISUAL MECHANISMS : X. MODIFICATIONS OF THE FLICKER RESPONSE CONTOUR, AND THE SIGNIFICANCE OF THE AVIAN PECTEN Crozier, W. J. Wolf, Ernst J Gen Physiol Article 1. When there is projected on the retina (man, monocularly) the shadow of a grid which forms a visual field in several distinct pieces (not including the fovea in the present tests), the ordinary properties of the flicker recognition contour (F vs. log I) as a function of the light-time cycle fraction (t(L)) can be markedly disturbed. In the present experiments flicker was produced by the rotation of a cylinder with opaque vertical stripes. In the absence of such a grid shadow the "cone" segments of the contours form a set in which F(max.) and the abscissa of inflection are opposite but rectilinear functions of t(L), while the third parameter of the probability integral (σ'(log I)) remains constant. This is the case also with diverse other animals tested. In the data with the grid, however, analysis shows that even for low values of t(L) (up to 0.50) there occurs an enhancement of the production of elements of neural effect, so that F(max.) rises rather than falls as ordinarily with increase of t(L), although σ'(log I) stays constant and hence the total number of acting units is presumed not to change. This constitutes valid evidence for neural integration of effects due to the illumination of separated retinal patches. Beginning at t(L) = 0.75, and at 0.90, the slope of the "cone" curve is sharply increased, and the maximum F is far above its position in the absence of the grid. The decrease of σ'(log I) (the slope constant) signifies, in terms of other information, an increase in the number of acting cone units. The abscissa of inflection is also much lowered, relatively, whereas without the grid it increases as t(L) is made larger. These effects correspond subjectively to the fact that at the end-point flicker is most pronounced, on the "cone" curve, along the edges of the grid shadow where contrast is particularly evident with the longer light-times. The "rod" portion of the F - log I contour is not specifically affected by the presence of the grid shadow. Its form is obtainable at t(L) = 0.90 free from the influence of summating "cone" contributions, because then almost no overlapping occurs. Analysis shows that when overlapping does occur a certain number of rod units are inhibited by concurrent cone excitation, and that the mean contribution of elements of neural action from each of the non-inhibited units is also reduced to an extent depending on the degree of overlap. The isolated "rod" curve at t(L) = 0.90 is quite accurately in the form of a probability integral. The data thus give a new experimental proof of the occurrence of two distinct but interlocking populations of visual effects, and experimentally justify the analytical procedures which have been used to separate them. 2. The changing form of the F - log I contour as a function of t(L), produced in man when the illuminated field is divided into parts by a shadow pattern, is normally found with the bird Taeniopygia castenotis (Gould), the zebra finch. The retina has elements of one general structural type (cones), and the F - log I contour is a simplex symmetrical probability integral. The eye of this bird has a large, complex, and darkly pigmented pecten, which casts a foliated shadow on the retina. The change in form of the F - log I curve occurs with t(L) above 0,50, and at t(L) = 0.90 is quite extreme. It is more pronounced than the one that is secured in the human data with the particular grid we have used, but there is no doubt that it could be mimicked completely by the use of other grids. The increase of flicker acuity due to the pecten shadow is considerable, when the dark spaces are brief relative to the light. The evidence thus confirms the suggestion (Menner) drawn from comparative natural history that the visual significance of the avian pecten might be to increase the sensory effect of small moving images. It is theoretically important that (as in the human experiment) this may be brought about by an actual decrease of effective retinal area illuminated. It is also significant theoretically that despite the presence of shadows of pecten or of grid, and of the sensory influences thus introduced, the probability integral formulation remains effective. The Rockefeller University Press 1944-03-20 /pmc/articles/PMC2238017/ /pubmed/19873387 Text en Copyright © Copyright, 1944, by The Rockefeller Institute for Medical Research This article is distributed under the terms of an Attribution–Noncommercial–Share Alike–No Mirror Sites license for the first six months after the publication date (see http://www.rupress.org/terms). After six months it is available under a Creative Commons License (Attribution–Noncommercial–Share Alike 4.0 Unported license, as described at http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/).
spellingShingle Article
Crozier, W. J.
Wolf, Ernst
THEORY AND MEASUREMENT OF VISUAL MECHANISMS : X. MODIFICATIONS OF THE FLICKER RESPONSE CONTOUR, AND THE SIGNIFICANCE OF THE AVIAN PECTEN
title THEORY AND MEASUREMENT OF VISUAL MECHANISMS : X. MODIFICATIONS OF THE FLICKER RESPONSE CONTOUR, AND THE SIGNIFICANCE OF THE AVIAN PECTEN
title_full THEORY AND MEASUREMENT OF VISUAL MECHANISMS : X. MODIFICATIONS OF THE FLICKER RESPONSE CONTOUR, AND THE SIGNIFICANCE OF THE AVIAN PECTEN
title_fullStr THEORY AND MEASUREMENT OF VISUAL MECHANISMS : X. MODIFICATIONS OF THE FLICKER RESPONSE CONTOUR, AND THE SIGNIFICANCE OF THE AVIAN PECTEN
title_full_unstemmed THEORY AND MEASUREMENT OF VISUAL MECHANISMS : X. MODIFICATIONS OF THE FLICKER RESPONSE CONTOUR, AND THE SIGNIFICANCE OF THE AVIAN PECTEN
title_short THEORY AND MEASUREMENT OF VISUAL MECHANISMS : X. MODIFICATIONS OF THE FLICKER RESPONSE CONTOUR, AND THE SIGNIFICANCE OF THE AVIAN PECTEN
title_sort theory and measurement of visual mechanisms : x. modifications of the flicker response contour, and the significance of the avian pecten
topic Article
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2238017/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19873387
work_keys_str_mv AT crozierwj theoryandmeasurementofvisualmechanismsxmodificationsoftheflickerresponsecontourandthesignificanceoftheavianpecten
AT wolfernst theoryandmeasurementofvisualmechanismsxmodificationsoftheflickerresponsecontourandthesignificanceoftheavianpecten