Cargando…

THEORY AND MEASUREMENT OF VISUAL MECHANISMS : XI. ON FLICKER WITH SUBDIVIDED FIELDS

Flicker contours for a square image of 3° visual angle, centered 6° on the temporal side of the fovea, the light sectored at a focus, are strikingly modified if the same illuminated area is arranged in four squares separated by a narrow opaque cross. The "cone" curves are made much steeper...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Crozier, W. J., Wolf, Ernst
Formato: Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: The Rockefeller University Press 1944
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2238022/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19873392
_version_ 1782150443769528320
author Crozier, W. J.
Wolf, Ernst
author_facet Crozier, W. J.
Wolf, Ernst
author_sort Crozier, W. J.
collection PubMed
description Flicker contours for a square image of 3° visual angle, centered 6° on the temporal side of the fovea, the light sectored at a focus, are strikingly modified if the same illuminated area is arranged in four squares separated by a narrow opaque cross. The "cone" curves are made much steeper, and their abscissae of inflection (τ' are at higher intensities; F(max.) is not greatly changed, but alters less with change of light-time fraction in the flash cycle (t(L)). This modification is accompanied by a great enlargement of the scotopic segment of the duplex curves, consistent with the theory of the integrative relations of neural effects in the two groups of units involved. The changes are not consistent with the view that flicker end-points are determined by the activation of retinal cells with a fixed spatial distribution of invariable thresholds. At t(L) = 0.50 the 3° subdivided area gives very nearly the same contour as does a square 6° x 6°, with the same total perimeter of light-dark separation; the "edge effect" thus suggested is complicated by differences in the dependence of F(max.) and τ' upon t(L). When an image pattern is produced by a grid of light bars separated by equally broad opaque spaces (10° x 10° over-all, centered at the fovea), the photopic flicker contours are made very steep and their midpoints are situated at quite low intensities, while the "rod" contribution tends to be more completely fused with the "cone" than is found for fields not subdivided. However, instead of a progressive increase of τ' with t(L) the curves for t(L) = 0.75 and 0.90 lie respectively below that for t(L) = 0.25 and 0.50 for a field of four broader stripes (1.43°) and both are below t(L) = 0.25 for a field of seven narrower stripes (0.77°). These latter changes are discussed in terms of the participation of subsidiary phenomena involving so called "γ movement." It is pointed out that since in these data σ(1)/I(m) is for each set of conditions a statistically constant quantity with a characteristic breadth of scatter σ(σ), it is possible to calculate a "coefficient of internal correlation" r which is a function of the conditions (as: image area, location, wave length of light, structure of image, light-time fraction) and which describes a property of any entire contour. The changes in r, as a function of the conditions of flicker excitation, reflect changes in the neural organization responsible for the liminal discrimination of flicker. It is shown that as consequence of simple changes in the image field, three parameters, as of the probability summation, are required for the description of a simplex flicker contour—since each of these is independently modifiable as to its magnitude and in its dependence on the light-time fraction. Subdivision of the image, with light sectored at a focus, produces in part only the changes in the flicker contour which we have earlier labelled the "pecten effect." In the latter, with light not sectored at a focus but with bar images moving across a field with inclined fixed opaque bars, the "cone" slope (dF/d log I) is sharply increased for t(L) > 0.50, but not below t(L) = 0.50, and the value of τ' is much less than it "should be." Consequently, the change in contrast brought about by the moving contact of light/dark borders is the significant factor in the "pecten effect," not simply pulsatile interruption of the light.
format Text
id pubmed-2238022
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 1944
publisher The Rockefeller University Press
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-22380222008-04-23 THEORY AND MEASUREMENT OF VISUAL MECHANISMS : XI. ON FLICKER WITH SUBDIVIDED FIELDS Crozier, W. J. Wolf, Ernst J Gen Physiol Article Flicker contours for a square image of 3° visual angle, centered 6° on the temporal side of the fovea, the light sectored at a focus, are strikingly modified if the same illuminated area is arranged in four squares separated by a narrow opaque cross. The "cone" curves are made much steeper, and their abscissae of inflection (τ' are at higher intensities; F(max.) is not greatly changed, but alters less with change of light-time fraction in the flash cycle (t(L)). This modification is accompanied by a great enlargement of the scotopic segment of the duplex curves, consistent with the theory of the integrative relations of neural effects in the two groups of units involved. The changes are not consistent with the view that flicker end-points are determined by the activation of retinal cells with a fixed spatial distribution of invariable thresholds. At t(L) = 0.50 the 3° subdivided area gives very nearly the same contour as does a square 6° x 6°, with the same total perimeter of light-dark separation; the "edge effect" thus suggested is complicated by differences in the dependence of F(max.) and τ' upon t(L). When an image pattern is produced by a grid of light bars separated by equally broad opaque spaces (10° x 10° over-all, centered at the fovea), the photopic flicker contours are made very steep and their midpoints are situated at quite low intensities, while the "rod" contribution tends to be more completely fused with the "cone" than is found for fields not subdivided. However, instead of a progressive increase of τ' with t(L) the curves for t(L) = 0.75 and 0.90 lie respectively below that for t(L) = 0.25 and 0.50 for a field of four broader stripes (1.43°) and both are below t(L) = 0.25 for a field of seven narrower stripes (0.77°). These latter changes are discussed in terms of the participation of subsidiary phenomena involving so called "γ movement." It is pointed out that since in these data σ(1)/I(m) is for each set of conditions a statistically constant quantity with a characteristic breadth of scatter σ(σ), it is possible to calculate a "coefficient of internal correlation" r which is a function of the conditions (as: image area, location, wave length of light, structure of image, light-time fraction) and which describes a property of any entire contour. The changes in r, as a function of the conditions of flicker excitation, reflect changes in the neural organization responsible for the liminal discrimination of flicker. It is shown that as consequence of simple changes in the image field, three parameters, as of the probability summation, are required for the description of a simplex flicker contour—since each of these is independently modifiable as to its magnitude and in its dependence on the light-time fraction. Subdivision of the image, with light sectored at a focus, produces in part only the changes in the flicker contour which we have earlier labelled the "pecten effect." In the latter, with light not sectored at a focus but with bar images moving across a field with inclined fixed opaque bars, the "cone" slope (dF/d log I) is sharply increased for t(L) > 0.50, but not below t(L) = 0.50, and the value of τ' is much less than it "should be." Consequently, the change in contrast brought about by the moving contact of light/dark borders is the significant factor in the "pecten effect," not simply pulsatile interruption of the light. The Rockefeller University Press 1944-05-20 /pmc/articles/PMC2238022/ /pubmed/19873392 Text en Copyright © Copyright, 1944, by The Rockefeller Institute for Medical Research This article is distributed under the terms of an Attribution–Noncommercial–Share Alike–No Mirror Sites license for the first six months after the publication date (see http://www.rupress.org/terms). After six months it is available under a Creative Commons License (Attribution–Noncommercial–Share Alike 4.0 Unported license, as described at http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/).
spellingShingle Article
Crozier, W. J.
Wolf, Ernst
THEORY AND MEASUREMENT OF VISUAL MECHANISMS : XI. ON FLICKER WITH SUBDIVIDED FIELDS
title THEORY AND MEASUREMENT OF VISUAL MECHANISMS : XI. ON FLICKER WITH SUBDIVIDED FIELDS
title_full THEORY AND MEASUREMENT OF VISUAL MECHANISMS : XI. ON FLICKER WITH SUBDIVIDED FIELDS
title_fullStr THEORY AND MEASUREMENT OF VISUAL MECHANISMS : XI. ON FLICKER WITH SUBDIVIDED FIELDS
title_full_unstemmed THEORY AND MEASUREMENT OF VISUAL MECHANISMS : XI. ON FLICKER WITH SUBDIVIDED FIELDS
title_short THEORY AND MEASUREMENT OF VISUAL MECHANISMS : XI. ON FLICKER WITH SUBDIVIDED FIELDS
title_sort theory and measurement of visual mechanisms : xi. on flicker with subdivided fields
topic Article
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2238022/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19873392
work_keys_str_mv AT crozierwj theoryandmeasurementofvisualmechanismsxionflickerwithsubdividedfields
AT wolfernst theoryandmeasurementofvisualmechanismsxionflickerwithsubdividedfields