Cargando…

Motor coordination: when two have to act as one

Trying to pass someone walking toward you in a narrow corridor is a familiar example of a two-person motor game that requires coordination. In this study, we investigate coordination in sensorimotor tasks that correspond to classic coordination games with multiple Nash equilibria, such as “choosing...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Braun, Daniel A., Ortega, Pedro A., Wolpert, Daniel M.
Formato: Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: Springer-Verlag 2011
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3102209/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21455618
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-011-2642-y
_version_ 1782204360122433536
author Braun, Daniel A.
Ortega, Pedro A.
Wolpert, Daniel M.
author_facet Braun, Daniel A.
Ortega, Pedro A.
Wolpert, Daniel M.
author_sort Braun, Daniel A.
collection PubMed
description Trying to pass someone walking toward you in a narrow corridor is a familiar example of a two-person motor game that requires coordination. In this study, we investigate coordination in sensorimotor tasks that correspond to classic coordination games with multiple Nash equilibria, such as “choosing sides,” “stag hunt,” “chicken,” and “battle of sexes”. In these tasks, subjects made reaching movements reflecting their continuously evolving “decisions” while they received a continuous payoff in the form of a resistive force counteracting their movements. Successful coordination required two subjects to “choose” the same Nash equilibrium in this force-payoff landscape within a single reach. We found that on the majority of trials coordination was achieved. Compared to the proportion of trials in which miscoordination occurred, successful coordination was characterized by several distinct features: an increased mutual information between the players’ movement endpoints, an increased joint entropy during the movements, and by differences in the timing of the players’ responses. Moreover, we found that the probability of successful coordination depends on the players’ initial distance from the Nash equilibria. Our results suggest that two-person coordination arises naturally in motor interactions and is facilitated by favorable initial positions, stereotypical motor pattern, and differences in response times. ELECTRONIC SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL: The online version of this article (doi:10.1007/s00221-011-2642-y) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.
format Text
id pubmed-3102209
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2011
publisher Springer-Verlag
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-31022092011-07-14 Motor coordination: when two have to act as one Braun, Daniel A. Ortega, Pedro A. Wolpert, Daniel M. Exp Brain Res Research Article Trying to pass someone walking toward you in a narrow corridor is a familiar example of a two-person motor game that requires coordination. In this study, we investigate coordination in sensorimotor tasks that correspond to classic coordination games with multiple Nash equilibria, such as “choosing sides,” “stag hunt,” “chicken,” and “battle of sexes”. In these tasks, subjects made reaching movements reflecting their continuously evolving “decisions” while they received a continuous payoff in the form of a resistive force counteracting their movements. Successful coordination required two subjects to “choose” the same Nash equilibrium in this force-payoff landscape within a single reach. We found that on the majority of trials coordination was achieved. Compared to the proportion of trials in which miscoordination occurred, successful coordination was characterized by several distinct features: an increased mutual information between the players’ movement endpoints, an increased joint entropy during the movements, and by differences in the timing of the players’ responses. Moreover, we found that the probability of successful coordination depends on the players’ initial distance from the Nash equilibria. Our results suggest that two-person coordination arises naturally in motor interactions and is facilitated by favorable initial positions, stereotypical motor pattern, and differences in response times. ELECTRONIC SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL: The online version of this article (doi:10.1007/s00221-011-2642-y) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users. Springer-Verlag 2011-04-01 2011 /pmc/articles/PMC3102209/ /pubmed/21455618 http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-011-2642-y Text en © The Author(s) 2011 https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/ This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Noncommercial License which permits any noncommercial use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author(s) and source are credited.
spellingShingle Research Article
Braun, Daniel A.
Ortega, Pedro A.
Wolpert, Daniel M.
Motor coordination: when two have to act as one
title Motor coordination: when two have to act as one
title_full Motor coordination: when two have to act as one
title_fullStr Motor coordination: when two have to act as one
title_full_unstemmed Motor coordination: when two have to act as one
title_short Motor coordination: when two have to act as one
title_sort motor coordination: when two have to act as one
topic Research Article
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3102209/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21455618
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-011-2642-y
work_keys_str_mv AT braundaniela motorcoordinationwhentwohavetoactasone
AT ortegapedroa motorcoordinationwhentwohavetoactasone
AT wolpertdanielm motorcoordinationwhentwohavetoactasone