Cargando…
Substitution and pooling in crowding
Unless we fixate directly on it, it is hard to see an object among other objects. This breakdown in object recognition, called crowding, severely limits peripheral vision. The effect is more severe when objects are more similar. When observers mistake the identity of a target among flanker objects,...
Autores principales: | , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
Springer-Verlag
2011
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3283763/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22160819 http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13414-011-0229-0 |
_version_ | 1782224254005149696 |
---|---|
author | Freeman, Jeremy Chakravarthi, Ramakrishna Pelli, Denis G. |
author_facet | Freeman, Jeremy Chakravarthi, Ramakrishna Pelli, Denis G. |
author_sort | Freeman, Jeremy |
collection | PubMed |
description | Unless we fixate directly on it, it is hard to see an object among other objects. This breakdown in object recognition, called crowding, severely limits peripheral vision. The effect is more severe when objects are more similar. When observers mistake the identity of a target among flanker objects, they often report a flanker. Many have taken these flanker reports as evidence of internal substitution of the target by a flanker. Here, we ask observers to identify a target letter presented in between one similar and one dissimilar flanker letter. Simple substitution takes in only one letter, which is often the target but, by unwitting mistake, is sometimes a flanker. The opposite of substitution is pooling, which takes in more than one letter. Having taken only one letter, the substitution process knows only its identity, not its similarity to the target. Thus, it must report similar and dissimilar flankers equally often. Contrary to this prediction, the similar flanker is reported much more often than the dissimilar flanker, showing that rampant flanker substitution cannot account for most flanker reports. Mixture modeling shows that simple substitution can account for, at most, about half the trials. Pooling and nonpooling (simple substitution) together include all possible models of crowding. When observers are asked to identify a crowded object, at least half of their reports are pooled, based on a combination of information from target and flankers, rather than being based on a single letter. ELECTRONIC SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL: The online version of this article (doi:10.3758/s13414-011-0229-0) contains supplementary material. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-3283763 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2011 |
publisher | Springer-Verlag |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-32837632012-03-01 Substitution and pooling in crowding Freeman, Jeremy Chakravarthi, Ramakrishna Pelli, Denis G. Atten Percept Psychophys Article Unless we fixate directly on it, it is hard to see an object among other objects. This breakdown in object recognition, called crowding, severely limits peripheral vision. The effect is more severe when objects are more similar. When observers mistake the identity of a target among flanker objects, they often report a flanker. Many have taken these flanker reports as evidence of internal substitution of the target by a flanker. Here, we ask observers to identify a target letter presented in between one similar and one dissimilar flanker letter. Simple substitution takes in only one letter, which is often the target but, by unwitting mistake, is sometimes a flanker. The opposite of substitution is pooling, which takes in more than one letter. Having taken only one letter, the substitution process knows only its identity, not its similarity to the target. Thus, it must report similar and dissimilar flankers equally often. Contrary to this prediction, the similar flanker is reported much more often than the dissimilar flanker, showing that rampant flanker substitution cannot account for most flanker reports. Mixture modeling shows that simple substitution can account for, at most, about half the trials. Pooling and nonpooling (simple substitution) together include all possible models of crowding. When observers are asked to identify a crowded object, at least half of their reports are pooled, based on a combination of information from target and flankers, rather than being based on a single letter. ELECTRONIC SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL: The online version of this article (doi:10.3758/s13414-011-0229-0) contains supplementary material. Springer-Verlag 2011-12-09 2012 /pmc/articles/PMC3283763/ /pubmed/22160819 http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13414-011-0229-0 Text en © The Author(s) 2011 https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/ This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Noncommercial License which permits any noncommercial use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author(s) and source are credited. |
spellingShingle | Article Freeman, Jeremy Chakravarthi, Ramakrishna Pelli, Denis G. Substitution and pooling in crowding |
title | Substitution and pooling in crowding |
title_full | Substitution and pooling in crowding |
title_fullStr | Substitution and pooling in crowding |
title_full_unstemmed | Substitution and pooling in crowding |
title_short | Substitution and pooling in crowding |
title_sort | substitution and pooling in crowding |
topic | Article |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3283763/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22160819 http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13414-011-0229-0 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT freemanjeremy substitutionandpoolingincrowding AT chakravarthiramakrishna substitutionandpoolingincrowding AT pellidenisg substitutionandpoolingincrowding |