Cargando…

Alfred Binet and the Concept of Heterogeneous Orders

In a comment, hitherto unremarked upon, Alfred Binet, well known for constructing the first intelligence scale, claimed that his scale did not measure intelligence, but only enabled classification with respect to a hierarchy of intellectual qualities. Attempting to understand the reasoning behind th...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autor principal: Michell, Joel
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: Frontiers Research Foundation 2012
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3419461/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22912619
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00261
_version_ 1782240732036202496
author Michell, Joel
author_facet Michell, Joel
author_sort Michell, Joel
collection PubMed
description In a comment, hitherto unremarked upon, Alfred Binet, well known for constructing the first intelligence scale, claimed that his scale did not measure intelligence, but only enabled classification with respect to a hierarchy of intellectual qualities. Attempting to understand the reasoning behind this comment leads to an historical excursion, beginning with the ancient mathematician, Euclid and ending with the modern French philosopher, Henri Bergson. As Euclid explained (Heath, 1908), magnitudes constituting a given quantitative attribute are all of the same kind (i.e., homogeneous), but his criterion covered only extensive magnitudes. Duns Scotus (Cross, 1998) included intensive magnitudes by considering differences, which raised the possibility (later considered by Sutherland, 2004) of ordered attributes with heterogeneous differences between degrees (“heterogeneous orders”). Of necessity, such attributes are non-measurable. Subsequently, this became a basis for the “quantity objection” to psychological measurement, as developed first by Tannery (1875a,b) and then by Bergson (1889). It follows that for attributes investigated in science, there are three structural possibilities: (1) classificatory attributes (with heterogeneous differences between categories); (2) heterogeneous orders (with heterogeneous differences between degrees); and (3) quantitative attributes (with thoroughly homogeneous differences between magnitudes). Measurement is possible only with attributes of kind (3) and, as far as we know, psychological attributes are exclusively of kinds (1) or (2). However, contrary to the known facts, psychometricians, for their own special reasons insist that test scores provide measurements.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-3419461
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2012
publisher Frontiers Research Foundation
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-34194612012-08-21 Alfred Binet and the Concept of Heterogeneous Orders Michell, Joel Front Psychol Psychology In a comment, hitherto unremarked upon, Alfred Binet, well known for constructing the first intelligence scale, claimed that his scale did not measure intelligence, but only enabled classification with respect to a hierarchy of intellectual qualities. Attempting to understand the reasoning behind this comment leads to an historical excursion, beginning with the ancient mathematician, Euclid and ending with the modern French philosopher, Henri Bergson. As Euclid explained (Heath, 1908), magnitudes constituting a given quantitative attribute are all of the same kind (i.e., homogeneous), but his criterion covered only extensive magnitudes. Duns Scotus (Cross, 1998) included intensive magnitudes by considering differences, which raised the possibility (later considered by Sutherland, 2004) of ordered attributes with heterogeneous differences between degrees (“heterogeneous orders”). Of necessity, such attributes are non-measurable. Subsequently, this became a basis for the “quantity objection” to psychological measurement, as developed first by Tannery (1875a,b) and then by Bergson (1889). It follows that for attributes investigated in science, there are three structural possibilities: (1) classificatory attributes (with heterogeneous differences between categories); (2) heterogeneous orders (with heterogeneous differences between degrees); and (3) quantitative attributes (with thoroughly homogeneous differences between magnitudes). Measurement is possible only with attributes of kind (3) and, as far as we know, psychological attributes are exclusively of kinds (1) or (2). However, contrary to the known facts, psychometricians, for their own special reasons insist that test scores provide measurements. Frontiers Research Foundation 2012-08-15 /pmc/articles/PMC3419461/ /pubmed/22912619 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00261 Text en Copyright © 2012 Michell. http://www.frontiersin.org/licenseagreement This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in other forums, provided the original authors and source are credited and subject to any copyright notices concerning any third-party graphics etc.
spellingShingle Psychology
Michell, Joel
Alfred Binet and the Concept of Heterogeneous Orders
title Alfred Binet and the Concept of Heterogeneous Orders
title_full Alfred Binet and the Concept of Heterogeneous Orders
title_fullStr Alfred Binet and the Concept of Heterogeneous Orders
title_full_unstemmed Alfred Binet and the Concept of Heterogeneous Orders
title_short Alfred Binet and the Concept of Heterogeneous Orders
title_sort alfred binet and the concept of heterogeneous orders
topic Psychology
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3419461/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22912619
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00261
work_keys_str_mv AT michelljoel alfredbinetandtheconceptofheterogeneousorders