Cargando…

Emotion and Deliberative Reasoning in Moral Judgment

According to an influential dual-process model, a moral judgment is the outcome of a rapid, affect-laden process and a slower, deliberative process. If these outputs conflict, decision time is increased in order to resolve the conflict. Violations of deontological principles proscribing the use of p...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Cummins, Denise Dellarosa, Cummins, Robert C.
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: Frontiers Research Foundation 2012
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3433709/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22973255
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00328
_version_ 1782242350639087616
author Cummins, Denise Dellarosa
Cummins, Robert C.
author_facet Cummins, Denise Dellarosa
Cummins, Robert C.
author_sort Cummins, Denise Dellarosa
collection PubMed
description According to an influential dual-process model, a moral judgment is the outcome of a rapid, affect-laden process and a slower, deliberative process. If these outputs conflict, decision time is increased in order to resolve the conflict. Violations of deontological principles proscribing the use of personal force to inflict intentional harm are presumed to elicit negative affect which biases judgments early in the decision-making process. This model was tested in three experiments. Moral dilemmas were classified using (a) decision time and consensus as measures of system conflict and (b) the aforementioned deontological criteria. In Experiment 1, decision time was either unlimited or reduced. The dilemmas asked whether it was appropriate to take a morally questionable action to produce a “greater good” outcome. Limiting decision time reduced the proportion of utilitarian (“yes”) decisions, but contrary to the model’s predictions, (a) vignettes that involved more deontological violations logged faster decision times, and (b) violation of deontological principles was not predictive of decisional conflict profiles. Experiment 2 ruled out the possibility that time pressure simply makes people more like to say “no.” Participants made a first decision under time constraints and a second decision under no time constraints. One group was asked whether it was appropriate to take the morally questionable action while a second group was asked whether it was appropriate to refuse to take the action. The results replicated that of Experiment 1 regardless of whether “yes” or “no” constituted a utilitarian decision. In Experiment 3, participants rated the pleasantness of positive visual stimuli prior to making a decision. Contrary to the model’s predictions, the number of deontological decisions increased in the positive affect rating group compared to a group that engaged in a cognitive task or a control group that engaged in neither task. These results are consistent with the view that early moral judgments are influenced by affect. But they are inconsistent with the view that (a) violation of deontological principles are predictive of differences in early, affect-based judgment or that (b) engaging in tasks that are inconsistent with the negative emotional responses elicited by such violations diminishes their impact.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-3433709
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2012
publisher Frontiers Research Foundation
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-34337092012-09-12 Emotion and Deliberative Reasoning in Moral Judgment Cummins, Denise Dellarosa Cummins, Robert C. Front Psychol Psychology According to an influential dual-process model, a moral judgment is the outcome of a rapid, affect-laden process and a slower, deliberative process. If these outputs conflict, decision time is increased in order to resolve the conflict. Violations of deontological principles proscribing the use of personal force to inflict intentional harm are presumed to elicit negative affect which biases judgments early in the decision-making process. This model was tested in three experiments. Moral dilemmas were classified using (a) decision time and consensus as measures of system conflict and (b) the aforementioned deontological criteria. In Experiment 1, decision time was either unlimited or reduced. The dilemmas asked whether it was appropriate to take a morally questionable action to produce a “greater good” outcome. Limiting decision time reduced the proportion of utilitarian (“yes”) decisions, but contrary to the model’s predictions, (a) vignettes that involved more deontological violations logged faster decision times, and (b) violation of deontological principles was not predictive of decisional conflict profiles. Experiment 2 ruled out the possibility that time pressure simply makes people more like to say “no.” Participants made a first decision under time constraints and a second decision under no time constraints. One group was asked whether it was appropriate to take the morally questionable action while a second group was asked whether it was appropriate to refuse to take the action. The results replicated that of Experiment 1 regardless of whether “yes” or “no” constituted a utilitarian decision. In Experiment 3, participants rated the pleasantness of positive visual stimuli prior to making a decision. Contrary to the model’s predictions, the number of deontological decisions increased in the positive affect rating group compared to a group that engaged in a cognitive task or a control group that engaged in neither task. These results are consistent with the view that early moral judgments are influenced by affect. But they are inconsistent with the view that (a) violation of deontological principles are predictive of differences in early, affect-based judgment or that (b) engaging in tasks that are inconsistent with the negative emotional responses elicited by such violations diminishes their impact. Frontiers Research Foundation 2012-09-05 /pmc/articles/PMC3433709/ /pubmed/22973255 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00328 Text en Copyright © 2012 Cummins and Cummins. http://www.frontiersin.org/licenseagreement This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in other forums, provided the original authors and source are credited and subject to any copyright notices concerning any third-party graphics etc.
spellingShingle Psychology
Cummins, Denise Dellarosa
Cummins, Robert C.
Emotion and Deliberative Reasoning in Moral Judgment
title Emotion and Deliberative Reasoning in Moral Judgment
title_full Emotion and Deliberative Reasoning in Moral Judgment
title_fullStr Emotion and Deliberative Reasoning in Moral Judgment
title_full_unstemmed Emotion and Deliberative Reasoning in Moral Judgment
title_short Emotion and Deliberative Reasoning in Moral Judgment
title_sort emotion and deliberative reasoning in moral judgment
topic Psychology
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3433709/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22973255
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00328
work_keys_str_mv AT cumminsdenisedellarosa emotionanddeliberativereasoninginmoraljudgment
AT cumminsrobertc emotionanddeliberativereasoninginmoraljudgment