Cargando…
The Biological Origin of Linguistic Diversity
In contrast with animal communication systems, diversity is characteristic of almost every aspect of human language. Languages variously employ tones, clicks, or manual signs to signal differences in meaning; some languages lack the noun-verb distinction (e.g., Straits Salish), whereas others have a...
Autores principales: | , , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
Public Library of Science
2012
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3484145/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23118922 http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0048029 |
_version_ | 1782248110977712128 |
---|---|
author | Baronchelli, Andrea Chater, Nick Pastor-Satorras, Romualdo Christiansen, Morten H. |
author_facet | Baronchelli, Andrea Chater, Nick Pastor-Satorras, Romualdo Christiansen, Morten H. |
author_sort | Baronchelli, Andrea |
collection | PubMed |
description | In contrast with animal communication systems, diversity is characteristic of almost every aspect of human language. Languages variously employ tones, clicks, or manual signs to signal differences in meaning; some languages lack the noun-verb distinction (e.g., Straits Salish), whereas others have a proliferation of fine-grained syntactic categories (e.g., Tzeltal); and some languages do without morphology (e.g., Mandarin), while others pack a whole sentence into a single word (e.g., Cayuga). A challenge for evolutionary biology is to reconcile the diversity of languages with the high degree of biological uniformity of their speakers. Here, we model processes of language change and geographical dispersion and find a consistent pressure for flexible learning, irrespective of the language being spoken. This pressure arises because flexible learners can best cope with the observed high rates of linguistic change associated with divergent cultural evolution following human migration. Thus, rather than genetic adaptations for specific aspects of language, such as recursion, the coevolution of genes and fast-changing linguistic structure provides the biological basis for linguistic diversity. Only biological adaptations for flexible learning combined with cultural evolution can explain how each child has the potential to learn any human language. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-3484145 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2012 |
publisher | Public Library of Science |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-34841452012-11-01 The Biological Origin of Linguistic Diversity Baronchelli, Andrea Chater, Nick Pastor-Satorras, Romualdo Christiansen, Morten H. PLoS One Research Article In contrast with animal communication systems, diversity is characteristic of almost every aspect of human language. Languages variously employ tones, clicks, or manual signs to signal differences in meaning; some languages lack the noun-verb distinction (e.g., Straits Salish), whereas others have a proliferation of fine-grained syntactic categories (e.g., Tzeltal); and some languages do without morphology (e.g., Mandarin), while others pack a whole sentence into a single word (e.g., Cayuga). A challenge for evolutionary biology is to reconcile the diversity of languages with the high degree of biological uniformity of their speakers. Here, we model processes of language change and geographical dispersion and find a consistent pressure for flexible learning, irrespective of the language being spoken. This pressure arises because flexible learners can best cope with the observed high rates of linguistic change associated with divergent cultural evolution following human migration. Thus, rather than genetic adaptations for specific aspects of language, such as recursion, the coevolution of genes and fast-changing linguistic structure provides the biological basis for linguistic diversity. Only biological adaptations for flexible learning combined with cultural evolution can explain how each child has the potential to learn any human language. Public Library of Science 2012-10-30 /pmc/articles/PMC3484145/ /pubmed/23118922 http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0048029 Text en © 2012 Baronchelli et al http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are properly credited. |
spellingShingle | Research Article Baronchelli, Andrea Chater, Nick Pastor-Satorras, Romualdo Christiansen, Morten H. The Biological Origin of Linguistic Diversity |
title | The Biological Origin of Linguistic Diversity |
title_full | The Biological Origin of Linguistic Diversity |
title_fullStr | The Biological Origin of Linguistic Diversity |
title_full_unstemmed | The Biological Origin of Linguistic Diversity |
title_short | The Biological Origin of Linguistic Diversity |
title_sort | biological origin of linguistic diversity |
topic | Research Article |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3484145/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23118922 http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0048029 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT baronchelliandrea thebiologicaloriginoflinguisticdiversity AT chaternick thebiologicaloriginoflinguisticdiversity AT pastorsatorrasromualdo thebiologicaloriginoflinguisticdiversity AT christiansenmortenh thebiologicaloriginoflinguisticdiversity AT baronchelliandrea biologicaloriginoflinguisticdiversity AT chaternick biologicaloriginoflinguisticdiversity AT pastorsatorrasromualdo biologicaloriginoflinguisticdiversity AT christiansenmortenh biologicaloriginoflinguisticdiversity |