Cargando…
Please Don't! The Automatic Extrapolation of Dangerous Intentions
Facial emotions and emotional body postures can easily grab attention in social communication. In the context of faces, gaze has been shown as an important cue for orienting attention, but less is known for other important body parts such as hands. In the present study we investigated whether hands...
Autores principales: | , , , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
Public Library of Science
2012
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3498372/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23155444 http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0049011 |
_version_ | 1782249834916347904 |
---|---|
author | Tessari, Alessia Ottoboni, Giovanni Mazzatenta, Andrea Merla, Arcangelo Nicoletti, Roberto |
author_facet | Tessari, Alessia Ottoboni, Giovanni Mazzatenta, Andrea Merla, Arcangelo Nicoletti, Roberto |
author_sort | Tessari, Alessia |
collection | PubMed |
description | Facial emotions and emotional body postures can easily grab attention in social communication. In the context of faces, gaze has been shown as an important cue for orienting attention, but less is known for other important body parts such as hands. In the present study we investigated whether hands may orient attention due to the emotional features they convey. By implying motion in static photographs of hands, we aimed at furnishing observers with information about the intention to act and at testing if this interacted with the hand automatic coding. In this study, we compared neutral and frontal hands to emotionally threatening hands, rotated along their radial-ulnar axes in a Sidedness task (a Simon-like task based on automatic access to body representation). Results showed a Sidedness effect for both the palm and the back views with either neutral and emotional hands. More important, no difference was found between the two views for neutral hands, but it emerged in the case of the emotional hands: faster reaction times were found for the palm than the back view. The difference was ascribed to palm views' “offensive” pose: a source of threat that might have raised participants' arousal. This hypothesis was also supported by conscious evaluations of the dimensions of valence (pleasant-unpleasant) and arousal. Results are discussed in light of emotional feature coding. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-3498372 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2012 |
publisher | Public Library of Science |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-34983722012-11-15 Please Don't! The Automatic Extrapolation of Dangerous Intentions Tessari, Alessia Ottoboni, Giovanni Mazzatenta, Andrea Merla, Arcangelo Nicoletti, Roberto PLoS One Research Article Facial emotions and emotional body postures can easily grab attention in social communication. In the context of faces, gaze has been shown as an important cue for orienting attention, but less is known for other important body parts such as hands. In the present study we investigated whether hands may orient attention due to the emotional features they convey. By implying motion in static photographs of hands, we aimed at furnishing observers with information about the intention to act and at testing if this interacted with the hand automatic coding. In this study, we compared neutral and frontal hands to emotionally threatening hands, rotated along their radial-ulnar axes in a Sidedness task (a Simon-like task based on automatic access to body representation). Results showed a Sidedness effect for both the palm and the back views with either neutral and emotional hands. More important, no difference was found between the two views for neutral hands, but it emerged in the case of the emotional hands: faster reaction times were found for the palm than the back view. The difference was ascribed to palm views' “offensive” pose: a source of threat that might have raised participants' arousal. This hypothesis was also supported by conscious evaluations of the dimensions of valence (pleasant-unpleasant) and arousal. Results are discussed in light of emotional feature coding. Public Library of Science 2012-11-14 /pmc/articles/PMC3498372/ /pubmed/23155444 http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0049011 Text en © 2012 Tessari et al http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are properly credited. |
spellingShingle | Research Article Tessari, Alessia Ottoboni, Giovanni Mazzatenta, Andrea Merla, Arcangelo Nicoletti, Roberto Please Don't! The Automatic Extrapolation of Dangerous Intentions |
title | Please Don't! The Automatic Extrapolation of Dangerous Intentions |
title_full | Please Don't! The Automatic Extrapolation of Dangerous Intentions |
title_fullStr | Please Don't! The Automatic Extrapolation of Dangerous Intentions |
title_full_unstemmed | Please Don't! The Automatic Extrapolation of Dangerous Intentions |
title_short | Please Don't! The Automatic Extrapolation of Dangerous Intentions |
title_sort | please don't! the automatic extrapolation of dangerous intentions |
topic | Research Article |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3498372/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23155444 http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0049011 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT tessarialessia pleasedonttheautomaticextrapolationofdangerousintentions AT ottobonigiovanni pleasedonttheautomaticextrapolationofdangerousintentions AT mazzatentaandrea pleasedonttheautomaticextrapolationofdangerousintentions AT merlaarcangelo pleasedonttheautomaticextrapolationofdangerousintentions AT nicolettiroberto pleasedonttheautomaticextrapolationofdangerousintentions |