Cargando…

The Segmentation of Sub-Lexical Morphemes in English-Learning 15-Month-Olds

In most human languages, important components of linguistic structure are carried by affixes, also called bound morphemes. The affixes in a language comprise a relatively small but frequently occurring set of forms that surface as parts of words, but never occur without a stem. They combine producti...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autor principal: Mintz, Toben H.
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: Frontiers Media S.A. 2013
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3565375/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23390420
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00024
_version_ 1782258455036297216
author Mintz, Toben H.
author_facet Mintz, Toben H.
author_sort Mintz, Toben H.
collection PubMed
description In most human languages, important components of linguistic structure are carried by affixes, also called bound morphemes. The affixes in a language comprise a relatively small but frequently occurring set of forms that surface as parts of words, but never occur without a stem. They combine productively with word stems and other grammatical entities in systematic and predictable ways. For example, the English suffix -ing occurs on verb stems, and in combination with a form of the auxiliary verb be, marks the verb with progressive aspect (e.g., was walking). In acquiring a language, learners must acquire rules of combination for affixes. However, prior to learning these combinatorial rules, learners are faced with discovering what the sub-lexical forms are over which the rules operate. That is, they have to discover the bound morphemes themselves. It is not known when English-learners begin to analyze words into morphological units. Previous research with learners of English found evidence that 18-month-olds have started to learn the combinatorial rules involving bound morphemes, and that 15-month-olds have not. However, it is not known whether 15-month-olds nevertheless represent the morphemes as distinct entities. This present study demonstrates that when 15-month-olds process words that end in -ing, they segment the suffix from the word, but they do not do so with endings that are not morphemes. Eight-month olds do not show this capacity. Thus, 15-month-olds have already started to identify bound morphemes and actively use them in processing speech.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-3565375
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2013
publisher Frontiers Media S.A.
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-35653752013-02-06 The Segmentation of Sub-Lexical Morphemes in English-Learning 15-Month-Olds Mintz, Toben H. Front Psychol Psychology In most human languages, important components of linguistic structure are carried by affixes, also called bound morphemes. The affixes in a language comprise a relatively small but frequently occurring set of forms that surface as parts of words, but never occur without a stem. They combine productively with word stems and other grammatical entities in systematic and predictable ways. For example, the English suffix -ing occurs on verb stems, and in combination with a form of the auxiliary verb be, marks the verb with progressive aspect (e.g., was walking). In acquiring a language, learners must acquire rules of combination for affixes. However, prior to learning these combinatorial rules, learners are faced with discovering what the sub-lexical forms are over which the rules operate. That is, they have to discover the bound morphemes themselves. It is not known when English-learners begin to analyze words into morphological units. Previous research with learners of English found evidence that 18-month-olds have started to learn the combinatorial rules involving bound morphemes, and that 15-month-olds have not. However, it is not known whether 15-month-olds nevertheless represent the morphemes as distinct entities. This present study demonstrates that when 15-month-olds process words that end in -ing, they segment the suffix from the word, but they do not do so with endings that are not morphemes. Eight-month olds do not show this capacity. Thus, 15-month-olds have already started to identify bound morphemes and actively use them in processing speech. Frontiers Media S.A. 2013-02-06 /pmc/articles/PMC3565375/ /pubmed/23390420 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00024 Text en Copyright © 2013 Mintz. http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/ This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in other forums, provided the original authors and source are credited and subject to any copyright notices concerning any third-party graphics etc.
spellingShingle Psychology
Mintz, Toben H.
The Segmentation of Sub-Lexical Morphemes in English-Learning 15-Month-Olds
title The Segmentation of Sub-Lexical Morphemes in English-Learning 15-Month-Olds
title_full The Segmentation of Sub-Lexical Morphemes in English-Learning 15-Month-Olds
title_fullStr The Segmentation of Sub-Lexical Morphemes in English-Learning 15-Month-Olds
title_full_unstemmed The Segmentation of Sub-Lexical Morphemes in English-Learning 15-Month-Olds
title_short The Segmentation of Sub-Lexical Morphemes in English-Learning 15-Month-Olds
title_sort segmentation of sub-lexical morphemes in english-learning 15-month-olds
topic Psychology
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3565375/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23390420
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00024
work_keys_str_mv AT mintztobenh thesegmentationofsublexicalmorphemesinenglishlearning15montholds
AT mintztobenh segmentationofsublexicalmorphemesinenglishlearning15montholds