Cargando…

Embodied Cognition is Not What you Think it is

The most exciting hypothesis in cognitive science right now is the theory that cognition is embodied. Like all good ideas in cognitive science, however, embodiment immediately came to mean six different things. The most common definitions involve the straight-forward claim that “states of the body m...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Wilson, Andrew D., Golonka, Sabrina
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: Frontiers Media S.A. 2013
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3569617/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23408669
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00058
_version_ 1782258930226823168
author Wilson, Andrew D.
Golonka, Sabrina
author_facet Wilson, Andrew D.
Golonka, Sabrina
author_sort Wilson, Andrew D.
collection PubMed
description The most exciting hypothesis in cognitive science right now is the theory that cognition is embodied. Like all good ideas in cognitive science, however, embodiment immediately came to mean six different things. The most common definitions involve the straight-forward claim that “states of the body modify states of the mind.” However, the implications of embodiment are actually much more radical than this. If cognition can span the brain, body, and the environment, then the “states of mind” of disembodied cognitive science won’t exist to be modified. Cognition will instead be an extended system assembled from a broad array of resources. Taking embodiment seriously therefore requires both new methods and theory. Here we outline four key steps that research programs should follow in order to fully engage with the implications of embodiment. The first step is to conduct a task analysis, which characterizes from a first person perspective the specific task that a perceiving-acting cognitive agent is faced with. The second step is to identify the task-relevant resources the agent has access to in order to solve the task. These resources can span brain, body, and environment. The third step is to identify how the agent can assemble these resources into a system capable of solving the problem at hand. The last step is to test the agent’s performance to confirm that agent is actually using the solution identified in step 3. We explore these steps in more detail with reference to two useful examples (the outfielder problem and the A-not-B error), and introduce how to apply this analysis to the thorny question of language use. Embodied cognition is more than we think it is, and we have the tools we need to realize its full potential.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-3569617
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2013
publisher Frontiers Media S.A.
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-35696172013-02-13 Embodied Cognition is Not What you Think it is Wilson, Andrew D. Golonka, Sabrina Front Psychol Psychology The most exciting hypothesis in cognitive science right now is the theory that cognition is embodied. Like all good ideas in cognitive science, however, embodiment immediately came to mean six different things. The most common definitions involve the straight-forward claim that “states of the body modify states of the mind.” However, the implications of embodiment are actually much more radical than this. If cognition can span the brain, body, and the environment, then the “states of mind” of disembodied cognitive science won’t exist to be modified. Cognition will instead be an extended system assembled from a broad array of resources. Taking embodiment seriously therefore requires both new methods and theory. Here we outline four key steps that research programs should follow in order to fully engage with the implications of embodiment. The first step is to conduct a task analysis, which characterizes from a first person perspective the specific task that a perceiving-acting cognitive agent is faced with. The second step is to identify the task-relevant resources the agent has access to in order to solve the task. These resources can span brain, body, and environment. The third step is to identify how the agent can assemble these resources into a system capable of solving the problem at hand. The last step is to test the agent’s performance to confirm that agent is actually using the solution identified in step 3. We explore these steps in more detail with reference to two useful examples (the outfielder problem and the A-not-B error), and introduce how to apply this analysis to the thorny question of language use. Embodied cognition is more than we think it is, and we have the tools we need to realize its full potential. Frontiers Media S.A. 2013-02-12 /pmc/articles/PMC3569617/ /pubmed/23408669 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00058 Text en Copyright © 2013 Wilson and Golonka. http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/ This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in other forums, provided the original authors and source are credited and subject to any copyright notices concerning any third-party graphics etc.
spellingShingle Psychology
Wilson, Andrew D.
Golonka, Sabrina
Embodied Cognition is Not What you Think it is
title Embodied Cognition is Not What you Think it is
title_full Embodied Cognition is Not What you Think it is
title_fullStr Embodied Cognition is Not What you Think it is
title_full_unstemmed Embodied Cognition is Not What you Think it is
title_short Embodied Cognition is Not What you Think it is
title_sort embodied cognition is not what you think it is
topic Psychology
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3569617/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23408669
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00058
work_keys_str_mv AT wilsonandrewd embodiedcognitionisnotwhatyouthinkitis
AT golonkasabrina embodiedcognitionisnotwhatyouthinkitis