Cargando…

Seeing and Hearing a Word: Combining Eye and Ear Is More Efficient than Combining the Parts of a Word

To understand why human sensitivity for complex objects is so low, we study how word identification combines eye and ear or parts of a word (features, letters, syllables). Our observers identify printed and spoken words presented concurrently or separately. When researchers measure threshold (energy...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Dubois, Matthieu, Poeppel, David, Pelli, Denis G.
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: Public Library of Science 2013
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3667182/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23734220
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0064803
_version_ 1782271459571269632
author Dubois, Matthieu
Poeppel, David
Pelli, Denis G.
author_facet Dubois, Matthieu
Poeppel, David
Pelli, Denis G.
author_sort Dubois, Matthieu
collection PubMed
description To understand why human sensitivity for complex objects is so low, we study how word identification combines eye and ear or parts of a word (features, letters, syllables). Our observers identify printed and spoken words presented concurrently or separately. When researchers measure threshold (energy of the faintest visible or audible signal) they may report either sensitivity (one over the human threshold) or efficiency (ratio of the best possible threshold to the human threshold). When the best possible algorithm identifies an object (like a word) in noise, its threshold is independent of how many parts the object has. But, with human observers, efficiency depends on the task. In some tasks, human observers combine parts efficiently, needing hardly more energy to identify an object with more parts. In other tasks, they combine inefficiently, needing energy nearly proportional to the number of parts, over a 60∶1 range. Whether presented to eye or ear, efficiency for detecting a short sinusoid (tone or grating) with few features is a substantial 20%, while efficiency for identifying a word with many features is merely 1%. Why? We show that the low human sensitivity for words is a cost of combining their many parts. We report a dichotomy between inefficient combining of adjacent features and efficient combining across senses. Joining our results with a survey of the cue-combination literature reveals that cues combine efficiently only if they are perceived as aspects of the same object. Observers give different names to adjacent letters in a word, and combine them inefficiently. Observers give the same name to a word’s image and sound, and combine them efficiently. The brain’s machinery optimally combines only cues that are perceived as originating from the same object. Presumably such cues each find their own way through the brain to arrive at the same object representation.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-3667182
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2013
publisher Public Library of Science
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-36671822013-06-03 Seeing and Hearing a Word: Combining Eye and Ear Is More Efficient than Combining the Parts of a Word Dubois, Matthieu Poeppel, David Pelli, Denis G. PLoS One Research Article To understand why human sensitivity for complex objects is so low, we study how word identification combines eye and ear or parts of a word (features, letters, syllables). Our observers identify printed and spoken words presented concurrently or separately. When researchers measure threshold (energy of the faintest visible or audible signal) they may report either sensitivity (one over the human threshold) or efficiency (ratio of the best possible threshold to the human threshold). When the best possible algorithm identifies an object (like a word) in noise, its threshold is independent of how many parts the object has. But, with human observers, efficiency depends on the task. In some tasks, human observers combine parts efficiently, needing hardly more energy to identify an object with more parts. In other tasks, they combine inefficiently, needing energy nearly proportional to the number of parts, over a 60∶1 range. Whether presented to eye or ear, efficiency for detecting a short sinusoid (tone or grating) with few features is a substantial 20%, while efficiency for identifying a word with many features is merely 1%. Why? We show that the low human sensitivity for words is a cost of combining their many parts. We report a dichotomy between inefficient combining of adjacent features and efficient combining across senses. Joining our results with a survey of the cue-combination literature reveals that cues combine efficiently only if they are perceived as aspects of the same object. Observers give different names to adjacent letters in a word, and combine them inefficiently. Observers give the same name to a word’s image and sound, and combine them efficiently. The brain’s machinery optimally combines only cues that are perceived as originating from the same object. Presumably such cues each find their own way through the brain to arrive at the same object representation. Public Library of Science 2013-05-29 /pmc/articles/PMC3667182/ /pubmed/23734220 http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0064803 Text en © 2013 Dubois et al http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are properly credited.
spellingShingle Research Article
Dubois, Matthieu
Poeppel, David
Pelli, Denis G.
Seeing and Hearing a Word: Combining Eye and Ear Is More Efficient than Combining the Parts of a Word
title Seeing and Hearing a Word: Combining Eye and Ear Is More Efficient than Combining the Parts of a Word
title_full Seeing and Hearing a Word: Combining Eye and Ear Is More Efficient than Combining the Parts of a Word
title_fullStr Seeing and Hearing a Word: Combining Eye and Ear Is More Efficient than Combining the Parts of a Word
title_full_unstemmed Seeing and Hearing a Word: Combining Eye and Ear Is More Efficient than Combining the Parts of a Word
title_short Seeing and Hearing a Word: Combining Eye and Ear Is More Efficient than Combining the Parts of a Word
title_sort seeing and hearing a word: combining eye and ear is more efficient than combining the parts of a word
topic Research Article
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3667182/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23734220
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0064803
work_keys_str_mv AT duboismatthieu seeingandhearingawordcombiningeyeandearismoreefficientthancombiningthepartsofaword
AT poeppeldavid seeingandhearingawordcombiningeyeandearismoreefficientthancombiningthepartsofaword
AT pellidenisg seeingandhearingawordcombiningeyeandearismoreefficientthancombiningthepartsofaword