Cargando…
On why music changes what (we think) we taste
A pair of recently published studies demonstrate that what we happen to be listening to can sometimes change our perception (or, at the very least, our rating) of what we are eating or drinking. In one recent study, North (2012) showed that the emotional attributes (or connotation) of a piece of mus...
Autores principales: | , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
Pion
2013
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3677333/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23755358 http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/i0577ic |
_version_ | 1782272722214060032 |
---|---|
author | Spence, Charles Deroy, Ophelia |
author_facet | Spence, Charles Deroy, Ophelia |
author_sort | Spence, Charles |
collection | PubMed |
description | A pair of recently published studies demonstrate that what we happen to be listening to can sometimes change our perception (or, at the very least, our rating) of what we are eating or drinking. In one recent study, North (2012) showed that the emotional attributes (or connotation) of a piece of music could influence people's perception of red or white wine. Meanwhile, Crisinel et al. (2012) reported that listening to a lower-pitched soundscape can help to emphasize the bitter notes in a bittersweet toffee while listening to a soundscape with a higher pitch tends to bring out its sweetness. Although the most appropriate psychological and neuroscientific explanations for such crossmodal effects are still uncertain, we outline a number of possible alternatives for such intriguing, not to mention surprising, phenomena. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-3677333 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2013 |
publisher | Pion |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-36773332013-06-10 On why music changes what (we think) we taste Spence, Charles Deroy, Ophelia Iperception i-Comment A pair of recently published studies demonstrate that what we happen to be listening to can sometimes change our perception (or, at the very least, our rating) of what we are eating or drinking. In one recent study, North (2012) showed that the emotional attributes (or connotation) of a piece of music could influence people's perception of red or white wine. Meanwhile, Crisinel et al. (2012) reported that listening to a lower-pitched soundscape can help to emphasize the bitter notes in a bittersweet toffee while listening to a soundscape with a higher pitch tends to bring out its sweetness. Although the most appropriate psychological and neuroscientific explanations for such crossmodal effects are still uncertain, we outline a number of possible alternatives for such intriguing, not to mention surprising, phenomena. Pion 2013-04-16 /pmc/articles/PMC3677333/ /pubmed/23755358 http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/i0577ic Text en Copyright 2013 C Spence, O Deroy http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/3.0/ This open-access article is distributed under a Creative Commons Licence, which permits noncommercial use, distribution, and reproduction, provided the original author(s) and source are credited and no alterations are made. |
spellingShingle | i-Comment Spence, Charles Deroy, Ophelia On why music changes what (we think) we taste |
title | On why music changes what (we think) we taste |
title_full | On why music changes what (we think) we taste |
title_fullStr | On why music changes what (we think) we taste |
title_full_unstemmed | On why music changes what (we think) we taste |
title_short | On why music changes what (we think) we taste |
title_sort | on why music changes what (we think) we taste |
topic | i-Comment |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3677333/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23755358 http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/i0577ic |
work_keys_str_mv | AT spencecharles onwhymusicchangeswhatwethinkwetaste AT deroyophelia onwhymusicchangeswhatwethinkwetaste |