Cargando…

Children benefit from morphological relatedness when they learn to spell new words

Use of morphologically related words often helps in selecting among spellings of sounds in French. For instance, final /wa/ may be spelled oi (e.g., envoi “sendoff”), oit (e.g., exploit “exploit”), ois (e.g., siamois, “siamese”), or oie (e.g., joie “joy”). The morphologically complex word exploiter...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Pacton, Sébastien, Foulin, Jean Noël, Casalis, Séverine, Treiman, Rebecca
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: Frontiers Media S.A. 2013
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3790073/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24109464
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00696
_version_ 1782286540468125696
author Pacton, Sébastien
Foulin, Jean Noël
Casalis, Séverine
Treiman, Rebecca
author_facet Pacton, Sébastien
Foulin, Jean Noël
Casalis, Séverine
Treiman, Rebecca
author_sort Pacton, Sébastien
collection PubMed
description Use of morphologically related words often helps in selecting among spellings of sounds in French. For instance, final /wa/ may be spelled oi (e.g., envoi “sendoff”), oit (e.g., exploit “exploit”), ois (e.g., siamois, “siamese”), or oie (e.g., joie “joy”). The morphologically complex word exploiter “to exploit”, with a pronounced t, can be used to indicate that the stem exploit is spelled with a silent t. We asked whether 8-year-old children benefited from such cues to learn new spellings. Children read silently stories which included two target nonwords, one presented in an opaque condition and the other in a morphological condition. In the opaque condition, the sentence provided semantic information (e.g., a vensois is a musical instrument) but no morphological information that could justify the spelling of the target word's final sound. Such justification was available in the morphological condition (e.g., the vensoisist plays the vensois instrument, which justifies that vensois includes a final silent s). 30 min after having read the stories, children's orthographic learning was assessed by asking them to choose the correct spelling of each nonword from among three phonologically plausible alternatives (e.g., vensois, vensoit, vensoie). Children chose correct spellings more often in the morphological condition than the opaque condition, even though the root (vensois) had been presented equally often in both conditions. That is, children benefited from information about the spelling of the morphologically complex word to learn the spelling of the stem.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-3790073
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2013
publisher Frontiers Media S.A.
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-37900732013-10-09 Children benefit from morphological relatedness when they learn to spell new words Pacton, Sébastien Foulin, Jean Noël Casalis, Séverine Treiman, Rebecca Front Psychol Psychology Use of morphologically related words often helps in selecting among spellings of sounds in French. For instance, final /wa/ may be spelled oi (e.g., envoi “sendoff”), oit (e.g., exploit “exploit”), ois (e.g., siamois, “siamese”), or oie (e.g., joie “joy”). The morphologically complex word exploiter “to exploit”, with a pronounced t, can be used to indicate that the stem exploit is spelled with a silent t. We asked whether 8-year-old children benefited from such cues to learn new spellings. Children read silently stories which included two target nonwords, one presented in an opaque condition and the other in a morphological condition. In the opaque condition, the sentence provided semantic information (e.g., a vensois is a musical instrument) but no morphological information that could justify the spelling of the target word's final sound. Such justification was available in the morphological condition (e.g., the vensoisist plays the vensois instrument, which justifies that vensois includes a final silent s). 30 min after having read the stories, children's orthographic learning was assessed by asking them to choose the correct spelling of each nonword from among three phonologically plausible alternatives (e.g., vensois, vensoit, vensoie). Children chose correct spellings more often in the morphological condition than the opaque condition, even though the root (vensois) had been presented equally often in both conditions. That is, children benefited from information about the spelling of the morphologically complex word to learn the spelling of the stem. Frontiers Media S.A. 2013-10-04 /pmc/articles/PMC3790073/ /pubmed/24109464 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00696 Text en Copyright © 2013 Pacton, Foulin, Casalis and Treiman. http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/ This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
spellingShingle Psychology
Pacton, Sébastien
Foulin, Jean Noël
Casalis, Séverine
Treiman, Rebecca
Children benefit from morphological relatedness when they learn to spell new words
title Children benefit from morphological relatedness when they learn to spell new words
title_full Children benefit from morphological relatedness when they learn to spell new words
title_fullStr Children benefit from morphological relatedness when they learn to spell new words
title_full_unstemmed Children benefit from morphological relatedness when they learn to spell new words
title_short Children benefit from morphological relatedness when they learn to spell new words
title_sort children benefit from morphological relatedness when they learn to spell new words
topic Psychology
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3790073/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24109464
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00696
work_keys_str_mv AT pactonsebastien childrenbenefitfrommorphologicalrelatednesswhentheylearntospellnewwords
AT foulinjeannoel childrenbenefitfrommorphologicalrelatednesswhentheylearntospellnewwords
AT casalisseverine childrenbenefitfrommorphologicalrelatednesswhentheylearntospellnewwords
AT treimanrebecca childrenbenefitfrommorphologicalrelatednesswhentheylearntospellnewwords