Cargando…
Children benefit from morphological relatedness when they learn to spell new words
Use of morphologically related words often helps in selecting among spellings of sounds in French. For instance, final /wa/ may be spelled oi (e.g., envoi “sendoff”), oit (e.g., exploit “exploit”), ois (e.g., siamois, “siamese”), or oie (e.g., joie “joy”). The morphologically complex word exploiter...
Autores principales: | , , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
Frontiers Media S.A.
2013
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3790073/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24109464 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00696 |
_version_ | 1782286540468125696 |
---|---|
author | Pacton, Sébastien Foulin, Jean Noël Casalis, Séverine Treiman, Rebecca |
author_facet | Pacton, Sébastien Foulin, Jean Noël Casalis, Séverine Treiman, Rebecca |
author_sort | Pacton, Sébastien |
collection | PubMed |
description | Use of morphologically related words often helps in selecting among spellings of sounds in French. For instance, final /wa/ may be spelled oi (e.g., envoi “sendoff”), oit (e.g., exploit “exploit”), ois (e.g., siamois, “siamese”), or oie (e.g., joie “joy”). The morphologically complex word exploiter “to exploit”, with a pronounced t, can be used to indicate that the stem exploit is spelled with a silent t. We asked whether 8-year-old children benefited from such cues to learn new spellings. Children read silently stories which included two target nonwords, one presented in an opaque condition and the other in a morphological condition. In the opaque condition, the sentence provided semantic information (e.g., a vensois is a musical instrument) but no morphological information that could justify the spelling of the target word's final sound. Such justification was available in the morphological condition (e.g., the vensoisist plays the vensois instrument, which justifies that vensois includes a final silent s). 30 min after having read the stories, children's orthographic learning was assessed by asking them to choose the correct spelling of each nonword from among three phonologically plausible alternatives (e.g., vensois, vensoit, vensoie). Children chose correct spellings more often in the morphological condition than the opaque condition, even though the root (vensois) had been presented equally often in both conditions. That is, children benefited from information about the spelling of the morphologically complex word to learn the spelling of the stem. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-3790073 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2013 |
publisher | Frontiers Media S.A. |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-37900732013-10-09 Children benefit from morphological relatedness when they learn to spell new words Pacton, Sébastien Foulin, Jean Noël Casalis, Séverine Treiman, Rebecca Front Psychol Psychology Use of morphologically related words often helps in selecting among spellings of sounds in French. For instance, final /wa/ may be spelled oi (e.g., envoi “sendoff”), oit (e.g., exploit “exploit”), ois (e.g., siamois, “siamese”), or oie (e.g., joie “joy”). The morphologically complex word exploiter “to exploit”, with a pronounced t, can be used to indicate that the stem exploit is spelled with a silent t. We asked whether 8-year-old children benefited from such cues to learn new spellings. Children read silently stories which included two target nonwords, one presented in an opaque condition and the other in a morphological condition. In the opaque condition, the sentence provided semantic information (e.g., a vensois is a musical instrument) but no morphological information that could justify the spelling of the target word's final sound. Such justification was available in the morphological condition (e.g., the vensoisist plays the vensois instrument, which justifies that vensois includes a final silent s). 30 min after having read the stories, children's orthographic learning was assessed by asking them to choose the correct spelling of each nonword from among three phonologically plausible alternatives (e.g., vensois, vensoit, vensoie). Children chose correct spellings more often in the morphological condition than the opaque condition, even though the root (vensois) had been presented equally often in both conditions. That is, children benefited from information about the spelling of the morphologically complex word to learn the spelling of the stem. Frontiers Media S.A. 2013-10-04 /pmc/articles/PMC3790073/ /pubmed/24109464 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00696 Text en Copyright © 2013 Pacton, Foulin, Casalis and Treiman. http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/ This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms. |
spellingShingle | Psychology Pacton, Sébastien Foulin, Jean Noël Casalis, Séverine Treiman, Rebecca Children benefit from morphological relatedness when they learn to spell new words |
title | Children benefit from morphological relatedness when they learn to spell new words |
title_full | Children benefit from morphological relatedness when they learn to spell new words |
title_fullStr | Children benefit from morphological relatedness when they learn to spell new words |
title_full_unstemmed | Children benefit from morphological relatedness when they learn to spell new words |
title_short | Children benefit from morphological relatedness when they learn to spell new words |
title_sort | children benefit from morphological relatedness when they learn to spell new words |
topic | Psychology |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3790073/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24109464 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00696 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT pactonsebastien childrenbenefitfrommorphologicalrelatednesswhentheylearntospellnewwords AT foulinjeannoel childrenbenefitfrommorphologicalrelatednesswhentheylearntospellnewwords AT casalisseverine childrenbenefitfrommorphologicalrelatednesswhentheylearntospellnewwords AT treimanrebecca childrenbenefitfrommorphologicalrelatednesswhentheylearntospellnewwords |