Cargando…
How does graphotactic knowledge influence children's learning of new spellings?
Two experiments investigated whether and how the learning of spellings by French third graders is influenced by two graphotactic patterns: consonants cannot double in word-initial position (Experiment 1) and consonants cannot double after single consonants (Experiment 2). Children silently read mean...
Autores principales: | , , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
Frontiers Media S.A.
2013
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3790077/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24109466 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00701 |
_version_ | 1782286541434912768 |
---|---|
author | Pacton, Sébastien Sobaco, Amélie Fayol, Michel Treiman, Rebecca |
author_facet | Pacton, Sébastien Sobaco, Amélie Fayol, Michel Treiman, Rebecca |
author_sort | Pacton, Sébastien |
collection | PubMed |
description | Two experiments investigated whether and how the learning of spellings by French third graders is influenced by two graphotactic patterns: consonants cannot double in word-initial position (Experiment 1) and consonants cannot double after single consonants (Experiment 2). Children silently read meaningful texts that contained three types of novel spellings: no doublet (e.g., mupile, guprane), doublet in a legal position (e.g., muppile, gupprane), and doublet in an illegal position (e.g., mmupile, guprrane). Orthographic learning was assessed with a task of spelling to dictation. In both experiments, children recalled items without doublets better than items with doublets. In Experiment 1, children recalled spellings with a doublet in illegal word-initial position better than spellings with a doublet in legal word-medial position, and almost all misspellings involved the omission of the doublet. The fact that the graphotactic violation in an item like mmupile was in the salient initial position may explain why children often remembered both the presence and the position of the doublet. In Experiment 2, children recalled non-words with a doublet before a single consonant (legal, e.g., gupprane) better than those with a doublet after a single consonant (illegal, e.g., guprrane). Omission of the doublet was the most frequent error for both types of items. Children also made some transposition errors on items with a doublet after a single consonant, recalling for example gupprane instead of guprrane. These results suggest that, when a doublet is in the hard-to-remember medial position, children sometimes remember that an item contains a doublet but not which letter is doubled. Their knowledge that double consonants can occur before but not after single consonants leads to transposition errors on items like guprrane. These results shed new light on the conditions under which children use general knowledge about the graphotactic patterns of their writing system to reconstruct spellings. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-3790077 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2013 |
publisher | Frontiers Media S.A. |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-37900772013-10-09 How does graphotactic knowledge influence children's learning of new spellings? Pacton, Sébastien Sobaco, Amélie Fayol, Michel Treiman, Rebecca Front Psychol Psychology Two experiments investigated whether and how the learning of spellings by French third graders is influenced by two graphotactic patterns: consonants cannot double in word-initial position (Experiment 1) and consonants cannot double after single consonants (Experiment 2). Children silently read meaningful texts that contained three types of novel spellings: no doublet (e.g., mupile, guprane), doublet in a legal position (e.g., muppile, gupprane), and doublet in an illegal position (e.g., mmupile, guprrane). Orthographic learning was assessed with a task of spelling to dictation. In both experiments, children recalled items without doublets better than items with doublets. In Experiment 1, children recalled spellings with a doublet in illegal word-initial position better than spellings with a doublet in legal word-medial position, and almost all misspellings involved the omission of the doublet. The fact that the graphotactic violation in an item like mmupile was in the salient initial position may explain why children often remembered both the presence and the position of the doublet. In Experiment 2, children recalled non-words with a doublet before a single consonant (legal, e.g., gupprane) better than those with a doublet after a single consonant (illegal, e.g., guprrane). Omission of the doublet was the most frequent error for both types of items. Children also made some transposition errors on items with a doublet after a single consonant, recalling for example gupprane instead of guprrane. These results suggest that, when a doublet is in the hard-to-remember medial position, children sometimes remember that an item contains a doublet but not which letter is doubled. Their knowledge that double consonants can occur before but not after single consonants leads to transposition errors on items like guprrane. These results shed new light on the conditions under which children use general knowledge about the graphotactic patterns of their writing system to reconstruct spellings. Frontiers Media S.A. 2013-10-04 /pmc/articles/PMC3790077/ /pubmed/24109466 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00701 Text en Copyright © 2013 Pacton, Sobaco, Fayol and Treiman. http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/ This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms. |
spellingShingle | Psychology Pacton, Sébastien Sobaco, Amélie Fayol, Michel Treiman, Rebecca How does graphotactic knowledge influence children's learning of new spellings? |
title | How does graphotactic knowledge influence children's learning of new spellings? |
title_full | How does graphotactic knowledge influence children's learning of new spellings? |
title_fullStr | How does graphotactic knowledge influence children's learning of new spellings? |
title_full_unstemmed | How does graphotactic knowledge influence children's learning of new spellings? |
title_short | How does graphotactic knowledge influence children's learning of new spellings? |
title_sort | how does graphotactic knowledge influence children's learning of new spellings? |
topic | Psychology |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3790077/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24109466 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00701 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT pactonsebastien howdoesgraphotacticknowledgeinfluencechildrenslearningofnewspellings AT sobacoamelie howdoesgraphotacticknowledgeinfluencechildrenslearningofnewspellings AT fayolmichel howdoesgraphotacticknowledgeinfluencechildrenslearningofnewspellings AT treimanrebecca howdoesgraphotacticknowledgeinfluencechildrenslearningofnewspellings |