Cargando…
Visual perspective and the characteristics of mind wandering
When the mind wanders away from the here-and-now toward imaginary events, it typically does so from one of two visual vantage points—a first-person perspective (i.e., the world is seen as it is in everyday life) or a third-person perspective (i.e., the world is seen from the viewpoint of an outside...
Autores principales: | , , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
Frontiers Media S.A.
2013
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3793122/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24130538 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00699 |
_version_ | 1782286925723336704 |
---|---|
author | Christian, Brittany M. Miles, Lynden K. Parkinson, Carolyn Macrae, C. Neil |
author_facet | Christian, Brittany M. Miles, Lynden K. Parkinson, Carolyn Macrae, C. Neil |
author_sort | Christian, Brittany M. |
collection | PubMed |
description | When the mind wanders away from the here-and-now toward imaginary events, it typically does so from one of two visual vantage points—a first-person perspective (i.e., the world is seen as it is in everyday life) or a third-person perspective (i.e., the world is seen from the viewpoint of an outside observer). While extant evidence has detailed consequences that ensue from the utilization of these distinct points of view, less is known about their more basic properties. Here, we investigated the prevalence, demographics and qualities associated with the visual perspective that people spontaneously adopt when the mind wanders. The results from a cross-cultural survey (N = 400) revealed that almost half of the participants (46%) typically utilize a third-person perspective when mind wandering. Further, culture and gender were shown to impact the distribution of first- and third-person imagers. Specifically, a first-person perspective was more common among participants from Western nations and females, while participants from Eastern cultures resonated more strongly with a third-person perspective. Moreover, these factors were also shown to impact qualities (e.g., temporal locus, vividness) of mental imagery. Taken together, the current findings elucidate the prevalence of first- and third-person visual perspectives and detail individual differences that influence the qualia of mind wandering. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-3793122 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2013 |
publisher | Frontiers Media S.A. |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-37931222013-10-15 Visual perspective and the characteristics of mind wandering Christian, Brittany M. Miles, Lynden K. Parkinson, Carolyn Macrae, C. Neil Front Psychol Psychology When the mind wanders away from the here-and-now toward imaginary events, it typically does so from one of two visual vantage points—a first-person perspective (i.e., the world is seen as it is in everyday life) or a third-person perspective (i.e., the world is seen from the viewpoint of an outside observer). While extant evidence has detailed consequences that ensue from the utilization of these distinct points of view, less is known about their more basic properties. Here, we investigated the prevalence, demographics and qualities associated with the visual perspective that people spontaneously adopt when the mind wanders. The results from a cross-cultural survey (N = 400) revealed that almost half of the participants (46%) typically utilize a third-person perspective when mind wandering. Further, culture and gender were shown to impact the distribution of first- and third-person imagers. Specifically, a first-person perspective was more common among participants from Western nations and females, while participants from Eastern cultures resonated more strongly with a third-person perspective. Moreover, these factors were also shown to impact qualities (e.g., temporal locus, vividness) of mental imagery. Taken together, the current findings elucidate the prevalence of first- and third-person visual perspectives and detail individual differences that influence the qualia of mind wandering. Frontiers Media S.A. 2013-10-09 /pmc/articles/PMC3793122/ /pubmed/24130538 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00699 Text en Copyright © 2013 Christian, Miles, Parkinson and Macrae. http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/ This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms. |
spellingShingle | Psychology Christian, Brittany M. Miles, Lynden K. Parkinson, Carolyn Macrae, C. Neil Visual perspective and the characteristics of mind wandering |
title | Visual perspective and the characteristics of mind wandering |
title_full | Visual perspective and the characteristics of mind wandering |
title_fullStr | Visual perspective and the characteristics of mind wandering |
title_full_unstemmed | Visual perspective and the characteristics of mind wandering |
title_short | Visual perspective and the characteristics of mind wandering |
title_sort | visual perspective and the characteristics of mind wandering |
topic | Psychology |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3793122/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24130538 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00699 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT christianbrittanym visualperspectiveandthecharacteristicsofmindwandering AT mileslyndenk visualperspectiveandthecharacteristicsofmindwandering AT parkinsoncarolyn visualperspectiveandthecharacteristicsofmindwandering AT macraecneil visualperspectiveandthecharacteristicsofmindwandering |