Cargando…

Factors that determine directional constraint in ipsilateral hand–foot coordinated movements

In performing simultaneous rhythmic movements of the ipsilateral hand and foot, there are differences in the level of stability between same directional (stable) and opposite directional (unstable) movements. This is the directional constraint. In this study, we investigated three factors (“interact...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Nakagawa, Kento, Muraoka, Tetsuro, Kanosue, Kazuyuki
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: Blackwell Publishing Ltd 2013
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3841043/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24303179
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/phy2.108
_version_ 1782478591404015616
author Nakagawa, Kento
Muraoka, Tetsuro
Kanosue, Kazuyuki
author_facet Nakagawa, Kento
Muraoka, Tetsuro
Kanosue, Kazuyuki
author_sort Nakagawa, Kento
collection PubMed
description In performing simultaneous rhythmic movements of the ipsilateral hand and foot, there are differences in the level of stability between same directional (stable) and opposite directional (unstable) movements. This is the directional constraint. In this study, we investigated three factors (“interaction in efferent process,” “interaction of afferent signals,” and “error correction”) proposed to underlie for the directional constraint. We compared the performance of three tasks: (1) coordination of actively moved ipsilateral hand and foot, (2) active hand movement in coordination with passively moved foot, (3) active hand movement not coordinated with a passively moved foot. In each task, both same and opposite directional movements were executed. There was no difference between performance estimated with success rate for the first and second task. The directional constraint appeared in both tasks. Thus, the interaction in efferent processes, which was shown to be responsible for the directional constraint in bimanual coordination, was not involved with the directional constraint of ipsilateral hand–foot coordination. The directional constraint did not appear in the third task, which suggested that “interaction of afferent signals” also had no contribution. These results indicated that “error correction” must be the most critical of these factors for mediating the directional constraint in ipsilateral hand–foot coordinated movements.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-3841043
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2013
publisher Blackwell Publishing Ltd
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-38410432013-12-03 Factors that determine directional constraint in ipsilateral hand–foot coordinated movements Nakagawa, Kento Muraoka, Tetsuro Kanosue, Kazuyuki Physiol Rep Original Research In performing simultaneous rhythmic movements of the ipsilateral hand and foot, there are differences in the level of stability between same directional (stable) and opposite directional (unstable) movements. This is the directional constraint. In this study, we investigated three factors (“interaction in efferent process,” “interaction of afferent signals,” and “error correction”) proposed to underlie for the directional constraint. We compared the performance of three tasks: (1) coordination of actively moved ipsilateral hand and foot, (2) active hand movement in coordination with passively moved foot, (3) active hand movement not coordinated with a passively moved foot. In each task, both same and opposite directional movements were executed. There was no difference between performance estimated with success rate for the first and second task. The directional constraint appeared in both tasks. Thus, the interaction in efferent processes, which was shown to be responsible for the directional constraint in bimanual coordination, was not involved with the directional constraint of ipsilateral hand–foot coordination. The directional constraint did not appear in the third task, which suggested that “interaction of afferent signals” also had no contribution. These results indicated that “error correction” must be the most critical of these factors for mediating the directional constraint in ipsilateral hand–foot coordinated movements. Blackwell Publishing Ltd 2013-10 2013-10-20 /pmc/articles/PMC3841043/ /pubmed/24303179 http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/phy2.108 Text en © 2013 The Authors. Physiological Reports published by Wiley Periodicals, Inc. on behalf of the American Physiological Society and The Physiological Society http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.5/ Re-use of this article is permitted in accordance with the Creative Commons Deed, Attribution 2.5, which does not permit commercial exploitation.
spellingShingle Original Research
Nakagawa, Kento
Muraoka, Tetsuro
Kanosue, Kazuyuki
Factors that determine directional constraint in ipsilateral hand–foot coordinated movements
title Factors that determine directional constraint in ipsilateral hand–foot coordinated movements
title_full Factors that determine directional constraint in ipsilateral hand–foot coordinated movements
title_fullStr Factors that determine directional constraint in ipsilateral hand–foot coordinated movements
title_full_unstemmed Factors that determine directional constraint in ipsilateral hand–foot coordinated movements
title_short Factors that determine directional constraint in ipsilateral hand–foot coordinated movements
title_sort factors that determine directional constraint in ipsilateral hand–foot coordinated movements
topic Original Research
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3841043/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24303179
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/phy2.108
work_keys_str_mv AT nakagawakento factorsthatdeterminedirectionalconstraintinipsilateralhandfootcoordinatedmovements
AT muraokatetsuro factorsthatdeterminedirectionalconstraintinipsilateralhandfootcoordinatedmovements
AT kanosuekazuyuki factorsthatdeterminedirectionalconstraintinipsilateralhandfootcoordinatedmovements