Cargando…
Response terminated displays unload selective attention
Perceptual load theory successfully replaced the early vs. late selection debate by appealing to adaptive control over the efficiency of selective attention. Early selection is observed unless perceptual load (p-Load) is sufficiently low to grant attentional “spill-over” to task-irrelevant stimuli....
Autores principales: | , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
Frontiers Media S.A.
2013
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3872048/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24399983 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00967 |
_version_ | 1782296915645300736 |
---|---|
author | Roper, Zachary J. J. Vecera, Shaun P. |
author_facet | Roper, Zachary J. J. Vecera, Shaun P. |
author_sort | Roper, Zachary J. J. |
collection | PubMed |
description | Perceptual load theory successfully replaced the early vs. late selection debate by appealing to adaptive control over the efficiency of selective attention. Early selection is observed unless perceptual load (p-Load) is sufficiently low to grant attentional “spill-over” to task-irrelevant stimuli. Many studies exploring load theory have used limited display durations that perhaps impose artificial limits on encoding processes. We extended the exposure duration in a classic p-Load task to alleviate temporal encoding demands that may otherwise tax mnemonic consolidation processes. If the load effect arises from perceptual demands alone, then freeing-up available mnemonic resources by extending the exposure duration should have little effect. The results of Experiment 1 falsify this prediction. We observed a reliable flanker effect under high p-Load, response-terminated displays. Next, we orthogonally manipulated exposure duration and task-relevance. Counter-intuitively, we found that the likelihood of observing the flanker effect under high p-Load resides with the duration of the task-relevant array, not the flanker itself. We propose that stimulus and encoding demands interact to produce the load effect. Our account clarifies how task parameters differentially impinge upon cognitive processes to produce attentional “spill-over” by appealing to visual short-term memory as an additional processing bottleneck when stimuli are briefly presented. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-3872048 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2013 |
publisher | Frontiers Media S.A. |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-38720482014-01-07 Response terminated displays unload selective attention Roper, Zachary J. J. Vecera, Shaun P. Front Psychol Psychology Perceptual load theory successfully replaced the early vs. late selection debate by appealing to adaptive control over the efficiency of selective attention. Early selection is observed unless perceptual load (p-Load) is sufficiently low to grant attentional “spill-over” to task-irrelevant stimuli. Many studies exploring load theory have used limited display durations that perhaps impose artificial limits on encoding processes. We extended the exposure duration in a classic p-Load task to alleviate temporal encoding demands that may otherwise tax mnemonic consolidation processes. If the load effect arises from perceptual demands alone, then freeing-up available mnemonic resources by extending the exposure duration should have little effect. The results of Experiment 1 falsify this prediction. We observed a reliable flanker effect under high p-Load, response-terminated displays. Next, we orthogonally manipulated exposure duration and task-relevance. Counter-intuitively, we found that the likelihood of observing the flanker effect under high p-Load resides with the duration of the task-relevant array, not the flanker itself. We propose that stimulus and encoding demands interact to produce the load effect. Our account clarifies how task parameters differentially impinge upon cognitive processes to produce attentional “spill-over” by appealing to visual short-term memory as an additional processing bottleneck when stimuli are briefly presented. Frontiers Media S.A. 2013-12-24 /pmc/articles/PMC3872048/ /pubmed/24399983 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00967 Text en Copyright © 2013 Roper and Vecera. http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/ This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms. |
spellingShingle | Psychology Roper, Zachary J. J. Vecera, Shaun P. Response terminated displays unload selective attention |
title | Response terminated displays unload selective attention |
title_full | Response terminated displays unload selective attention |
title_fullStr | Response terminated displays unload selective attention |
title_full_unstemmed | Response terminated displays unload selective attention |
title_short | Response terminated displays unload selective attention |
title_sort | response terminated displays unload selective attention |
topic | Psychology |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3872048/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24399983 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00967 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT roperzacharyjj responseterminateddisplaysunloadselectiveattention AT vecerashaunp responseterminateddisplaysunloadselectiveattention |