Cargando…

Kinematics of Visually-Guided Eye Movements

One of the hallmarks of an eye movement that follows Listing’s law is the half-angle rule that says that the angular velocity of the eye tilts by half the angle of eccentricity of the line of sight relative to primary eye position. Since all visually-guided eye movements in the regime of far viewing...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Hess, Bernhard J. M., Thomassen, Jakob S.
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: Public Library of Science 2014
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3994052/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24751602
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0095234
_version_ 1782312658216681472
author Hess, Bernhard J. M.
Thomassen, Jakob S.
author_facet Hess, Bernhard J. M.
Thomassen, Jakob S.
author_sort Hess, Bernhard J. M.
collection PubMed
description One of the hallmarks of an eye movement that follows Listing’s law is the half-angle rule that says that the angular velocity of the eye tilts by half the angle of eccentricity of the line of sight relative to primary eye position. Since all visually-guided eye movements in the regime of far viewing follow Listing’s law (with the head still and upright), the question about its origin is of considerable importance. Here, we provide theoretical and experimental evidence that Listing’s law results from a unique motor strategy that allows minimizing ocular torsion while smoothly tracking objects of interest along any path in visual space. The strategy consists in compounding conventional ocular rotations in meridian planes, that is in horizontal, vertical and oblique directions (which are all torsion-free) with small linear displacements of the eye in the frontal plane. Such compound rotation-displacements of the eye can explain the kinematic paradox that the fixation point may rotate in one plane while the eye rotates in other planes. Its unique signature is the half-angle law in the position domain, which means that the rotation plane of the eye tilts by half-the angle of gaze eccentricity. We show that this law does not readily generalize to the velocity domain of visually-guided eye movements because the angular eye velocity is the sum of two terms, one associated with rotations in meridian planes and one associated with displacements of the eye in the frontal plane. While the first term does not depend on eye position the second term does depend on eye position. We show that compounded rotation - displacements perfectly predict the average smooth kinematics of the eye during steady- state pursuit in both the position and velocity domain.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-3994052
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2014
publisher Public Library of Science
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-39940522014-04-25 Kinematics of Visually-Guided Eye Movements Hess, Bernhard J. M. Thomassen, Jakob S. PLoS One Research Article One of the hallmarks of an eye movement that follows Listing’s law is the half-angle rule that says that the angular velocity of the eye tilts by half the angle of eccentricity of the line of sight relative to primary eye position. Since all visually-guided eye movements in the regime of far viewing follow Listing’s law (with the head still and upright), the question about its origin is of considerable importance. Here, we provide theoretical and experimental evidence that Listing’s law results from a unique motor strategy that allows minimizing ocular torsion while smoothly tracking objects of interest along any path in visual space. The strategy consists in compounding conventional ocular rotations in meridian planes, that is in horizontal, vertical and oblique directions (which are all torsion-free) with small linear displacements of the eye in the frontal plane. Such compound rotation-displacements of the eye can explain the kinematic paradox that the fixation point may rotate in one plane while the eye rotates in other planes. Its unique signature is the half-angle law in the position domain, which means that the rotation plane of the eye tilts by half-the angle of gaze eccentricity. We show that this law does not readily generalize to the velocity domain of visually-guided eye movements because the angular eye velocity is the sum of two terms, one associated with rotations in meridian planes and one associated with displacements of the eye in the frontal plane. While the first term does not depend on eye position the second term does depend on eye position. We show that compounded rotation - displacements perfectly predict the average smooth kinematics of the eye during steady- state pursuit in both the position and velocity domain. Public Library of Science 2014-04-21 /pmc/articles/PMC3994052/ /pubmed/24751602 http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0095234 Text en © 2014 Hess, Thomassen http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are properly credited.
spellingShingle Research Article
Hess, Bernhard J. M.
Thomassen, Jakob S.
Kinematics of Visually-Guided Eye Movements
title Kinematics of Visually-Guided Eye Movements
title_full Kinematics of Visually-Guided Eye Movements
title_fullStr Kinematics of Visually-Guided Eye Movements
title_full_unstemmed Kinematics of Visually-Guided Eye Movements
title_short Kinematics of Visually-Guided Eye Movements
title_sort kinematics of visually-guided eye movements
topic Research Article
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3994052/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24751602
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0095234
work_keys_str_mv AT hessbernhardjm kinematicsofvisuallyguidedeyemovements
AT thomassenjakobs kinematicsofvisuallyguidedeyemovements