Cargando…

Evidence for adjustable bandwidth orientation channels

The standard model of early vision claims that orientation and spatial frequency are encoded with multiple, quasi-independent channels that have fixed spatial frequency and orientation bandwidths. The standard model was developed using detection and discrimination data collected from experiments tha...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Taylor, Christopher P., Bennett, Patrick J., Sekuler, Allison B.
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: Frontiers Media S.A. 2014
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4054014/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24971069
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00578
_version_ 1782320486208765952
author Taylor, Christopher P.
Bennett, Patrick J.
Sekuler, Allison B.
author_facet Taylor, Christopher P.
Bennett, Patrick J.
Sekuler, Allison B.
author_sort Taylor, Christopher P.
collection PubMed
description The standard model of early vision claims that orientation and spatial frequency are encoded with multiple, quasi-independent channels that have fixed spatial frequency and orientation bandwidths. The standard model was developed using detection and discrimination data collected from experiments that used deterministic patterns such as Gabor patches and gratings used as stimuli. However, detection data from experiments using noise as a stimulus suggests that the visual system may use adjustable-bandwidth, rather than fixed-bandwidth, channels. In our previous work, we used classification images as a key piece of evidence against the hypothesis that pattern detection is based on the responses of channels with an adjustable spatial frequency bandwidth. Here we tested the hypothesis that channels with adjustable orientation bandwidths are used to detect two-dimensional, filtered noise targets that varied in orientation bandwidth and were presented in white noise. Consistent with our previous work that examined spatial frequency bandwidth, we found that detection thresholds were consistent with the hypothesis that observers sum information across a broad range of orientations nearly optimally: absolute efficiency for stimulus detection was 20–30% and approximately constant across a wide range of orientation bandwidths. Unlike what we found with spatial frequency bandwidth, the results of our classification image experiment were consistent with the hypothesis that the orientation bandwidth of internal filters were adjustable. Thus, for orientation summation, both detection thresholds and classification images support the adjustable channels hypothesis. Classification images also revealed hallmarks of inhibition or suppression from uninformative spatial frequencies and/or orientations. This work highlights the limitations of the standard model of summation for orientation. The standard model of orientation summation and tuning was chiefly developed with narrow-band stimuli that were not presented in noise, stimuli that are arguably less naturalistic than the variable bandwidth stimuli presented in noise used in our experiments. Finally, the disagreement between the results from our experiments on spatial frequency summation with the data presented in this paper suggests that orientation may be encoded more flexibly than spatial frequency channels.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-4054014
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2014
publisher Frontiers Media S.A.
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-40540142014-06-26 Evidence for adjustable bandwidth orientation channels Taylor, Christopher P. Bennett, Patrick J. Sekuler, Allison B. Front Psychol Psychology The standard model of early vision claims that orientation and spatial frequency are encoded with multiple, quasi-independent channels that have fixed spatial frequency and orientation bandwidths. The standard model was developed using detection and discrimination data collected from experiments that used deterministic patterns such as Gabor patches and gratings used as stimuli. However, detection data from experiments using noise as a stimulus suggests that the visual system may use adjustable-bandwidth, rather than fixed-bandwidth, channels. In our previous work, we used classification images as a key piece of evidence against the hypothesis that pattern detection is based on the responses of channels with an adjustable spatial frequency bandwidth. Here we tested the hypothesis that channels with adjustable orientation bandwidths are used to detect two-dimensional, filtered noise targets that varied in orientation bandwidth and were presented in white noise. Consistent with our previous work that examined spatial frequency bandwidth, we found that detection thresholds were consistent with the hypothesis that observers sum information across a broad range of orientations nearly optimally: absolute efficiency for stimulus detection was 20–30% and approximately constant across a wide range of orientation bandwidths. Unlike what we found with spatial frequency bandwidth, the results of our classification image experiment were consistent with the hypothesis that the orientation bandwidth of internal filters were adjustable. Thus, for orientation summation, both detection thresholds and classification images support the adjustable channels hypothesis. Classification images also revealed hallmarks of inhibition or suppression from uninformative spatial frequencies and/or orientations. This work highlights the limitations of the standard model of summation for orientation. The standard model of orientation summation and tuning was chiefly developed with narrow-band stimuli that were not presented in noise, stimuli that are arguably less naturalistic than the variable bandwidth stimuli presented in noise used in our experiments. Finally, the disagreement between the results from our experiments on spatial frequency summation with the data presented in this paper suggests that orientation may be encoded more flexibly than spatial frequency channels. Frontiers Media S.A. 2014-06-12 /pmc/articles/PMC4054014/ /pubmed/24971069 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00578 Text en Copyright © 2014 Taylor, Bennett and Sekuler. http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/ This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
spellingShingle Psychology
Taylor, Christopher P.
Bennett, Patrick J.
Sekuler, Allison B.
Evidence for adjustable bandwidth orientation channels
title Evidence for adjustable bandwidth orientation channels
title_full Evidence for adjustable bandwidth orientation channels
title_fullStr Evidence for adjustable bandwidth orientation channels
title_full_unstemmed Evidence for adjustable bandwidth orientation channels
title_short Evidence for adjustable bandwidth orientation channels
title_sort evidence for adjustable bandwidth orientation channels
topic Psychology
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4054014/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24971069
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00578
work_keys_str_mv AT taylorchristopherp evidenceforadjustablebandwidthorientationchannels
AT bennettpatrickj evidenceforadjustablebandwidthorientationchannels
AT sekulerallisonb evidenceforadjustablebandwidthorientationchannels