Cargando…
Using tools with real and imagined tool movements
When using lever tools, subjects have to deal with two, not necessarily concordant effects of their motor behavior: the body-related proximal effects, like tactile sensations from the moving hand, and/or more external distal effects, like the moving effect points of the lever. As a consequence, spat...
Autores principales: | , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
Frontiers Media S.A.
2014
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4060803/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24987379 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00515 |
_version_ | 1782321410394292224 |
---|---|
author | Müsseler, Jochen Wühr, Peter Ziessler, Michael |
author_facet | Müsseler, Jochen Wühr, Peter Ziessler, Michael |
author_sort | Müsseler, Jochen |
collection | PubMed |
description | When using lever tools, subjects have to deal with two, not necessarily concordant effects of their motor behavior: the body-related proximal effects, like tactile sensations from the moving hand, and/or more external distal effects, like the moving effect points of the lever. As a consequence, spatial compatibility relationships between stimulus (S; at which the effect points of the lever aim at), responding hand (R) and effect point of the lever (E) play a critical role in response generation. In the present study we examine whether the occurrence of compatibility effects needs real tool movements or whether a similar response pattern can be already evoked by pure mental imaginations of the tool effects. In general, response times and errors observed with real and imagined tool movements showed a similar pattern of results, but there were also differences. With incompatible relationships and thus more difficult tasks, response times were reduced with imagined tool movements than compared with real tool movements. On the contrary, with compatible relationships and thus high overlap between proximal and distal action effects, response times were increased with imagined tool movements. Results are only in parts consistent with the ideomotor theory of motor control. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-4060803 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2014 |
publisher | Frontiers Media S.A. |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-40608032014-07-01 Using tools with real and imagined tool movements Müsseler, Jochen Wühr, Peter Ziessler, Michael Front Psychol Psychology When using lever tools, subjects have to deal with two, not necessarily concordant effects of their motor behavior: the body-related proximal effects, like tactile sensations from the moving hand, and/or more external distal effects, like the moving effect points of the lever. As a consequence, spatial compatibility relationships between stimulus (S; at which the effect points of the lever aim at), responding hand (R) and effect point of the lever (E) play a critical role in response generation. In the present study we examine whether the occurrence of compatibility effects needs real tool movements or whether a similar response pattern can be already evoked by pure mental imaginations of the tool effects. In general, response times and errors observed with real and imagined tool movements showed a similar pattern of results, but there were also differences. With incompatible relationships and thus more difficult tasks, response times were reduced with imagined tool movements than compared with real tool movements. On the contrary, with compatible relationships and thus high overlap between proximal and distal action effects, response times were increased with imagined tool movements. Results are only in parts consistent with the ideomotor theory of motor control. Frontiers Media S.A. 2014-06-17 /pmc/articles/PMC4060803/ /pubmed/24987379 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00515 Text en Copyright © 2014 Müsseler, Wühr and Ziessler. http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/ This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms. |
spellingShingle | Psychology Müsseler, Jochen Wühr, Peter Ziessler, Michael Using tools with real and imagined tool movements |
title | Using tools with real and imagined tool movements |
title_full | Using tools with real and imagined tool movements |
title_fullStr | Using tools with real and imagined tool movements |
title_full_unstemmed | Using tools with real and imagined tool movements |
title_short | Using tools with real and imagined tool movements |
title_sort | using tools with real and imagined tool movements |
topic | Psychology |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4060803/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24987379 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00515 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT musselerjochen usingtoolswithrealandimaginedtoolmovements AT wuhrpeter usingtoolswithrealandimaginedtoolmovements AT ziesslermichael usingtoolswithrealandimaginedtoolmovements |