Cargando…
Detecting and correcting partial errors: Evidence for efficient control without conscious access
Appropriate reactions to erroneous actions are essential to keeping behavior adaptive. Erring, however, is not an all-or-none process: electromyographic (EMG) recordings of the responding muscles have revealed that covert incorrect response activations (termed “partial errors”) occur on a proportion...
Autores principales: | , , , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
Springer US
2013
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4125819/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24347086 http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13415-013-0232-0 |
_version_ | 1782329807193767936 |
---|---|
author | Rochet, N. Spieser, L. Casini, L. Hasbroucq, T. Burle, B. |
author_facet | Rochet, N. Spieser, L. Casini, L. Hasbroucq, T. Burle, B. |
author_sort | Rochet, N. |
collection | PubMed |
description | Appropriate reactions to erroneous actions are essential to keeping behavior adaptive. Erring, however, is not an all-or-none process: electromyographic (EMG) recordings of the responding muscles have revealed that covert incorrect response activations (termed “partial errors”) occur on a proportion of overtly correct trials. The occurrence of such “partial errors” shows that incorrect response activations could be corrected online, before turning into overt errors. In the present study, we showed that, unlike overt errors, such “partial errors” are poorly consciously detected by participants, who could report only one third of their partial errors. Two parameters of the partial errors were found to predict detection: the surface of the incorrect EMG burst (larger for detected) and the correction time (between the incorrect and correct EMG onsets; longer for detected). These two parameters provided independent information. The correct(ive) responses associated with detected partial errors were larger than the “pure-correct” ones, and this increase was likely a consequence, rather than a cause, of the detection. The respective impacts of the two parameters predicting detection (incorrect surface and correction time), along with the underlying physiological processes subtending partial-error detection, are discussed. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-4125819 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2013 |
publisher | Springer US |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-41258192014-08-08 Detecting and correcting partial errors: Evidence for efficient control without conscious access Rochet, N. Spieser, L. Casini, L. Hasbroucq, T. Burle, B. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci Article Appropriate reactions to erroneous actions are essential to keeping behavior adaptive. Erring, however, is not an all-or-none process: electromyographic (EMG) recordings of the responding muscles have revealed that covert incorrect response activations (termed “partial errors”) occur on a proportion of overtly correct trials. The occurrence of such “partial errors” shows that incorrect response activations could be corrected online, before turning into overt errors. In the present study, we showed that, unlike overt errors, such “partial errors” are poorly consciously detected by participants, who could report only one third of their partial errors. Two parameters of the partial errors were found to predict detection: the surface of the incorrect EMG burst (larger for detected) and the correction time (between the incorrect and correct EMG onsets; longer for detected). These two parameters provided independent information. The correct(ive) responses associated with detected partial errors were larger than the “pure-correct” ones, and this increase was likely a consequence, rather than a cause, of the detection. The respective impacts of the two parameters predicting detection (incorrect surface and correction time), along with the underlying physiological processes subtending partial-error detection, are discussed. Springer US 2013-12-18 2014 /pmc/articles/PMC4125819/ /pubmed/24347086 http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13415-013-0232-0 Text en © The Author(s) 2013 https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0/ Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License which permits any use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author(s) and the source are credited. |
spellingShingle | Article Rochet, N. Spieser, L. Casini, L. Hasbroucq, T. Burle, B. Detecting and correcting partial errors: Evidence for efficient control without conscious access |
title | Detecting and correcting partial errors: Evidence for efficient control without conscious access |
title_full | Detecting and correcting partial errors: Evidence for efficient control without conscious access |
title_fullStr | Detecting and correcting partial errors: Evidence for efficient control without conscious access |
title_full_unstemmed | Detecting and correcting partial errors: Evidence for efficient control without conscious access |
title_short | Detecting and correcting partial errors: Evidence for efficient control without conscious access |
title_sort | detecting and correcting partial errors: evidence for efficient control without conscious access |
topic | Article |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4125819/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24347086 http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13415-013-0232-0 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT rochetn detectingandcorrectingpartialerrorsevidenceforefficientcontrolwithoutconsciousaccess AT spieserl detectingandcorrectingpartialerrorsevidenceforefficientcontrolwithoutconsciousaccess AT casinil detectingandcorrectingpartialerrorsevidenceforefficientcontrolwithoutconsciousaccess AT hasbroucqt detectingandcorrectingpartialerrorsevidenceforefficientcontrolwithoutconsciousaccess AT burleb detectingandcorrectingpartialerrorsevidenceforefficientcontrolwithoutconsciousaccess |