Cargando…

Is Rumination after Bereavement Linked with Loss Avoidance? Evidence from Eye-Tracking

Rumination is a risk factor in adjustment to bereavement. It is associated with and predicts psychopathology after loss. Yet, the function of rumination in bereavement remains unclear. In the past, researchers often assumed rumination to be a maladaptive confrontation process. However, based on cogn...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Eisma, Maarten C., Schut, Henk A. W., Stroebe, Margaret S., van den Bout, Jan, Stroebe, Wolfgang, Boelen, Paul A.
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: Public Library of Science 2014
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4139328/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25140524
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0104980
_version_ 1782331347179667456
author Eisma, Maarten C.
Schut, Henk A. W.
Stroebe, Margaret S.
van den Bout, Jan
Stroebe, Wolfgang
Boelen, Paul A.
author_facet Eisma, Maarten C.
Schut, Henk A. W.
Stroebe, Margaret S.
van den Bout, Jan
Stroebe, Wolfgang
Boelen, Paul A.
author_sort Eisma, Maarten C.
collection PubMed
description Rumination is a risk factor in adjustment to bereavement. It is associated with and predicts psychopathology after loss. Yet, the function of rumination in bereavement remains unclear. In the past, researchers often assumed rumination to be a maladaptive confrontation process. However, based on cognitive avoidance theories of worry in generalised anxiety disorder (GAD) and rumination after post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), others have suggested that rumination may serve to avoid painful aspects of the loss, thereby contributing to complicated grief. To examine if rumination is linked with loss avoidance, an eye-tracking study was conducted with 54 bereaved individuals (27 high and 27 low ruminators). On 24 trials, participants looked for 10 seconds at a picture of the deceased and a picture of a stranger, randomly combined with negative, neutral or loss-related words. High ruminators were expected to show initial vigilance followed by subsequent disengagement for loss stimuli (i.e., picture deceased with a loss word) in the first 1500 ms. Additionally, we expected high ruminators to avoid these loss stimuli and to show attentional preference for non-loss-related negative stimuli (i.e., picture stranger with a negative word) on longer exposure durations (1500–10000 ms). Contrary to expectations, we found no evidence for an effect of rumination on vigilance and disengagement of loss stimuli in the first 1500 ms. However, in the 1500–10000 ms interval, high ruminators showed shorter gaze times for loss stimuli and longer gaze times for negative (and neutral) non-loss-related stimuli, even when controlling for depression and complicated grief symptom levels. Effects of rumination on average fixation times mirrored these findings. This suggests that rumination and loss avoidance are closely associated. A potential clinical implication is that rumination and grief complications after bereavement may be reduced through the use of exposure and acceptance-based therapeutic techniques.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-4139328
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2014
publisher Public Library of Science
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-41393282014-08-25 Is Rumination after Bereavement Linked with Loss Avoidance? Evidence from Eye-Tracking Eisma, Maarten C. Schut, Henk A. W. Stroebe, Margaret S. van den Bout, Jan Stroebe, Wolfgang Boelen, Paul A. PLoS One Research Article Rumination is a risk factor in adjustment to bereavement. It is associated with and predicts psychopathology after loss. Yet, the function of rumination in bereavement remains unclear. In the past, researchers often assumed rumination to be a maladaptive confrontation process. However, based on cognitive avoidance theories of worry in generalised anxiety disorder (GAD) and rumination after post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), others have suggested that rumination may serve to avoid painful aspects of the loss, thereby contributing to complicated grief. To examine if rumination is linked with loss avoidance, an eye-tracking study was conducted with 54 bereaved individuals (27 high and 27 low ruminators). On 24 trials, participants looked for 10 seconds at a picture of the deceased and a picture of a stranger, randomly combined with negative, neutral or loss-related words. High ruminators were expected to show initial vigilance followed by subsequent disengagement for loss stimuli (i.e., picture deceased with a loss word) in the first 1500 ms. Additionally, we expected high ruminators to avoid these loss stimuli and to show attentional preference for non-loss-related negative stimuli (i.e., picture stranger with a negative word) on longer exposure durations (1500–10000 ms). Contrary to expectations, we found no evidence for an effect of rumination on vigilance and disengagement of loss stimuli in the first 1500 ms. However, in the 1500–10000 ms interval, high ruminators showed shorter gaze times for loss stimuli and longer gaze times for negative (and neutral) non-loss-related stimuli, even when controlling for depression and complicated grief symptom levels. Effects of rumination on average fixation times mirrored these findings. This suggests that rumination and loss avoidance are closely associated. A potential clinical implication is that rumination and grief complications after bereavement may be reduced through the use of exposure and acceptance-based therapeutic techniques. Public Library of Science 2014-08-20 /pmc/articles/PMC4139328/ /pubmed/25140524 http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0104980 Text en © 2014 Eisma et al http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are properly credited.
spellingShingle Research Article
Eisma, Maarten C.
Schut, Henk A. W.
Stroebe, Margaret S.
van den Bout, Jan
Stroebe, Wolfgang
Boelen, Paul A.
Is Rumination after Bereavement Linked with Loss Avoidance? Evidence from Eye-Tracking
title Is Rumination after Bereavement Linked with Loss Avoidance? Evidence from Eye-Tracking
title_full Is Rumination after Bereavement Linked with Loss Avoidance? Evidence from Eye-Tracking
title_fullStr Is Rumination after Bereavement Linked with Loss Avoidance? Evidence from Eye-Tracking
title_full_unstemmed Is Rumination after Bereavement Linked with Loss Avoidance? Evidence from Eye-Tracking
title_short Is Rumination after Bereavement Linked with Loss Avoidance? Evidence from Eye-Tracking
title_sort is rumination after bereavement linked with loss avoidance? evidence from eye-tracking
topic Research Article
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4139328/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25140524
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0104980
work_keys_str_mv AT eismamaartenc isruminationafterbereavementlinkedwithlossavoidanceevidencefromeyetracking
AT schuthenkaw isruminationafterbereavementlinkedwithlossavoidanceevidencefromeyetracking
AT stroebemargarets isruminationafterbereavementlinkedwithlossavoidanceevidencefromeyetracking
AT vandenboutjan isruminationafterbereavementlinkedwithlossavoidanceevidencefromeyetracking
AT stroebewolfgang isruminationafterbereavementlinkedwithlossavoidanceevidencefromeyetracking
AT boelenpaula isruminationafterbereavementlinkedwithlossavoidanceevidencefromeyetracking