Cargando…
Leisure activity associated with cognitive ability level, but not cognitive change
Although activity participation is promoted as cognitively protective, critical questions of causality remain. In a cohort followed every 5 years from age 75 to 85 years, potential reciprocal associations between level and change in leisure activity participation and level and change in cognitive ab...
Autores principales: | , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
Frontiers Media S.A.
2014
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4196477/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25352824 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01176 |
_version_ | 1782339483426881536 |
---|---|
author | Gow, Alan J. Avlund, Kirsten Mortensen, Erik L. |
author_facet | Gow, Alan J. Avlund, Kirsten Mortensen, Erik L. |
author_sort | Gow, Alan J. |
collection | PubMed |
description | Although activity participation is promoted as cognitively protective, critical questions of causality remain. In a cohort followed every 5 years from age 75 to 85 years, potential reciprocal associations between level and change in leisure activity participation and level and change in cognitive abilities were examined. Participants in the Glostrup 1914 Cohort, a longitudinal study of aging, completed standardized cognitive ability tests and reported their leisure activity participation (11 activities defined a leisure activity score) at ages 75, 80, and 85. Higher leisure activity was associated with higher cognitive ability (significant correlations ranged from 0.15 to 0.31, p < 0.05). Between ages 75 and 85, participation in leisure activities and cognitive ability declined significantly. Growth curve models, which provided latent variables for level of and 10-year change in both leisure activity and cognitive ability, confirmed the positive association between levels of leisure activity and cognitive ability (path coefficient = 0.36, p < 0.001); however, neither leisure activity level nor change in leisure activity were associated with cognitive change. Although a positive association between leisure activity and cognitive ability was reported—the likely precedents of this are discussed—there was no evidence that a higher level or maintenance of leisure activity was protective against cognitive decline across a 10-year follow-up. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-4196477 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2014 |
publisher | Frontiers Media S.A. |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-41964772014-10-28 Leisure activity associated with cognitive ability level, but not cognitive change Gow, Alan J. Avlund, Kirsten Mortensen, Erik L. Front Psychol Psychology Although activity participation is promoted as cognitively protective, critical questions of causality remain. In a cohort followed every 5 years from age 75 to 85 years, potential reciprocal associations between level and change in leisure activity participation and level and change in cognitive abilities were examined. Participants in the Glostrup 1914 Cohort, a longitudinal study of aging, completed standardized cognitive ability tests and reported their leisure activity participation (11 activities defined a leisure activity score) at ages 75, 80, and 85. Higher leisure activity was associated with higher cognitive ability (significant correlations ranged from 0.15 to 0.31, p < 0.05). Between ages 75 and 85, participation in leisure activities and cognitive ability declined significantly. Growth curve models, which provided latent variables for level of and 10-year change in both leisure activity and cognitive ability, confirmed the positive association between levels of leisure activity and cognitive ability (path coefficient = 0.36, p < 0.001); however, neither leisure activity level nor change in leisure activity were associated with cognitive change. Although a positive association between leisure activity and cognitive ability was reported—the likely precedents of this are discussed—there was no evidence that a higher level or maintenance of leisure activity was protective against cognitive decline across a 10-year follow-up. Frontiers Media S.A. 2014-10-14 /pmc/articles/PMC4196477/ /pubmed/25352824 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01176 Text en Copyright © 2014 Gow, Avlund and Mortensen. http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms. |
spellingShingle | Psychology Gow, Alan J. Avlund, Kirsten Mortensen, Erik L. Leisure activity associated with cognitive ability level, but not cognitive change |
title | Leisure activity associated with cognitive ability level, but not cognitive change |
title_full | Leisure activity associated with cognitive ability level, but not cognitive change |
title_fullStr | Leisure activity associated with cognitive ability level, but not cognitive change |
title_full_unstemmed | Leisure activity associated with cognitive ability level, but not cognitive change |
title_short | Leisure activity associated with cognitive ability level, but not cognitive change |
title_sort | leisure activity associated with cognitive ability level, but not cognitive change |
topic | Psychology |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4196477/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25352824 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01176 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT gowalanj leisureactivityassociatedwithcognitiveabilitylevelbutnotcognitivechange AT avlundkirsten leisureactivityassociatedwithcognitiveabilitylevelbutnotcognitivechange AT mortensenerikl leisureactivityassociatedwithcognitiveabilitylevelbutnotcognitivechange |