Cargando…
Learning rate and temperament in a high predation risk environment
Living in challenging environments can influence the behavior of animals in a number of ways. For instance, populations of prey fish that experience frequent, nonlethal interactions with predators have a high proportion of individuals that express greater reaction to risk and increased activity and...
Autores principales: | , , , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
2014
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4207961/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25270336 http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00442-014-3099-z |
_version_ | 1782341063662370816 |
---|---|
author | DePasquale, C. Wagner, T. Archard, G. A. Ferguson, B. Braithwaite, V. A. |
author_facet | DePasquale, C. Wagner, T. Archard, G. A. Ferguson, B. Braithwaite, V. A. |
author_sort | DePasquale, C. |
collection | PubMed |
description | Living in challenging environments can influence the behavior of animals in a number of ways. For instance, populations of prey fish that experience frequent, nonlethal interactions with predators have a high proportion of individuals that express greater reaction to risk and increased activity and exploration—collectively known as temperament traits. Temperament traits are often correlated, such that individuals that are risk-prone also tend to be active and explore more. Spatial learning, which requires the integration of many sensory cues, has also been shown to vary in fish exposed to different levels of predation threat. Fish from areas of low predation risk learn to solve spatial tasks faster than fish from high predation areas. However, it is not yet known whether simpler forms of learning, such as learning associations between two events, are similarly influenced. Simple forms of associative learning are likely to be affected by temperament because a willingness to approach and explore novel situations could provide animals with a learning advantage. However, it is possible that routine-forming and inflexible traits associated with risk-prone and increased exploratory behavior may act in the opposite way and make risk-prone individuals poorer at learning associations. To investigate this, we measured temperament in Panamanian bishop fish (Brachyrhaphis episcopi) sampled from a site known to contain many predators. The B. episcopi were then tested with an associative learning task. Within this population, fish that explored more were faster at learning a cue that predicted access to food, indicating a link between temperament and basic learning abilities. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-4207961 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2014 |
publisher | Springer Berlin Heidelberg |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-42079612014-10-28 Learning rate and temperament in a high predation risk environment DePasquale, C. Wagner, T. Archard, G. A. Ferguson, B. Braithwaite, V. A. Oecologia Special Topic: Ecophysiological Effects of Predation Risk Living in challenging environments can influence the behavior of animals in a number of ways. For instance, populations of prey fish that experience frequent, nonlethal interactions with predators have a high proportion of individuals that express greater reaction to risk and increased activity and exploration—collectively known as temperament traits. Temperament traits are often correlated, such that individuals that are risk-prone also tend to be active and explore more. Spatial learning, which requires the integration of many sensory cues, has also been shown to vary in fish exposed to different levels of predation threat. Fish from areas of low predation risk learn to solve spatial tasks faster than fish from high predation areas. However, it is not yet known whether simpler forms of learning, such as learning associations between two events, are similarly influenced. Simple forms of associative learning are likely to be affected by temperament because a willingness to approach and explore novel situations could provide animals with a learning advantage. However, it is possible that routine-forming and inflexible traits associated with risk-prone and increased exploratory behavior may act in the opposite way and make risk-prone individuals poorer at learning associations. To investigate this, we measured temperament in Panamanian bishop fish (Brachyrhaphis episcopi) sampled from a site known to contain many predators. The B. episcopi were then tested with an associative learning task. Within this population, fish that explored more were faster at learning a cue that predicted access to food, indicating a link between temperament and basic learning abilities. Springer Berlin Heidelberg 2014-10-02 2014 /pmc/articles/PMC4207961/ /pubmed/25270336 http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00442-014-3099-z Text en © The Author(s) 2014 https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License which permits any use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author(s) and the source are credited. |
spellingShingle | Special Topic: Ecophysiological Effects of Predation Risk DePasquale, C. Wagner, T. Archard, G. A. Ferguson, B. Braithwaite, V. A. Learning rate and temperament in a high predation risk environment |
title | Learning rate and temperament in a high predation risk environment |
title_full | Learning rate and temperament in a high predation risk environment |
title_fullStr | Learning rate and temperament in a high predation risk environment |
title_full_unstemmed | Learning rate and temperament in a high predation risk environment |
title_short | Learning rate and temperament in a high predation risk environment |
title_sort | learning rate and temperament in a high predation risk environment |
topic | Special Topic: Ecophysiological Effects of Predation Risk |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4207961/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25270336 http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00442-014-3099-z |
work_keys_str_mv | AT depasqualec learningrateandtemperamentinahighpredationriskenvironment AT wagnert learningrateandtemperamentinahighpredationriskenvironment AT archardga learningrateandtemperamentinahighpredationriskenvironment AT fergusonb learningrateandtemperamentinahighpredationriskenvironment AT braithwaiteva learningrateandtemperamentinahighpredationriskenvironment |