Cargando…
Is the Glass Half Full or Half Empty? How to Reverse the Effect of Glass Elongation on the Volume Poured
To reduce the volume of drinks and the risk of overconsumption, health professionals recommend the use of tall skinny instead of short wide glasses. Yet the results of the present study contradict this health advice. Participants who generously filled up a glass with lemonade served 9% more in tall...
Autores principales: | , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
Public Library of Science
2014
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4208726/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25343252 http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0109374 |
_version_ | 1782341163860099072 |
---|---|
author | Caljouw, Simone R. van Wijck, Ruud |
author_facet | Caljouw, Simone R. van Wijck, Ruud |
author_sort | Caljouw, Simone R. |
collection | PubMed |
description | To reduce the volume of drinks and the risk of overconsumption, health professionals recommend the use of tall skinny instead of short wide glasses. Yet the results of the present study contradict this health advice. Participants who generously filled up a glass with lemonade served 9% more in tall narrow compared with short wide glasses (p<0.05). In addition, when pouring a small amount (i.e., a shot), participants poured 3% more in a short wide than in a tall narrow glass (p<0.05). Elongation may bias the perceived volume that is poured but also the perceived volume of the free space in the glass. We hypothesised that shifting attention from the bottom to the brim of the glass when filling it close to capacity might reverse the glass elongation effect on the quantity poured. This hypothesis was tested, by investigating two pouring tasks that differed in the required focus of attention. When the instruction was to match a reference volume, participants poured more liquid in the short wide compared with the tall narrow glass (p<0.05). The effect of glass elongation on poured volume was the opposite when the instruction was to leave space in the glasses for the reference volume. It seems likely that task and individual factors affect the pourer's viewing strategy and thus may determine the direction of the glass elongation effect on the volume poured. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-4208726 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2014 |
publisher | Public Library of Science |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-42087262014-10-27 Is the Glass Half Full or Half Empty? How to Reverse the Effect of Glass Elongation on the Volume Poured Caljouw, Simone R. van Wijck, Ruud PLoS One Research Article To reduce the volume of drinks and the risk of overconsumption, health professionals recommend the use of tall skinny instead of short wide glasses. Yet the results of the present study contradict this health advice. Participants who generously filled up a glass with lemonade served 9% more in tall narrow compared with short wide glasses (p<0.05). In addition, when pouring a small amount (i.e., a shot), participants poured 3% more in a short wide than in a tall narrow glass (p<0.05). Elongation may bias the perceived volume that is poured but also the perceived volume of the free space in the glass. We hypothesised that shifting attention from the bottom to the brim of the glass when filling it close to capacity might reverse the glass elongation effect on the quantity poured. This hypothesis was tested, by investigating two pouring tasks that differed in the required focus of attention. When the instruction was to match a reference volume, participants poured more liquid in the short wide compared with the tall narrow glass (p<0.05). The effect of glass elongation on poured volume was the opposite when the instruction was to leave space in the glasses for the reference volume. It seems likely that task and individual factors affect the pourer's viewing strategy and thus may determine the direction of the glass elongation effect on the volume poured. Public Library of Science 2014-10-24 /pmc/articles/PMC4208726/ /pubmed/25343252 http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0109374 Text en © 2014 Caljouw, van Wijck http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are properly credited. |
spellingShingle | Research Article Caljouw, Simone R. van Wijck, Ruud Is the Glass Half Full or Half Empty? How to Reverse the Effect of Glass Elongation on the Volume Poured |
title | Is the Glass Half Full or Half Empty? How to Reverse the Effect of Glass Elongation on the Volume Poured |
title_full | Is the Glass Half Full or Half Empty? How to Reverse the Effect of Glass Elongation on the Volume Poured |
title_fullStr | Is the Glass Half Full or Half Empty? How to Reverse the Effect of Glass Elongation on the Volume Poured |
title_full_unstemmed | Is the Glass Half Full or Half Empty? How to Reverse the Effect of Glass Elongation on the Volume Poured |
title_short | Is the Glass Half Full or Half Empty? How to Reverse the Effect of Glass Elongation on the Volume Poured |
title_sort | is the glass half full or half empty? how to reverse the effect of glass elongation on the volume poured |
topic | Research Article |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4208726/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25343252 http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0109374 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT caljouwsimoner istheglasshalffullorhalfemptyhowtoreversetheeffectofglasselongationonthevolumepoured AT vanwijckruud istheglasshalffullorhalfemptyhowtoreversetheeffectofglasselongationonthevolumepoured |