Cargando…
Testing for baseline differences in randomized controlled trials: an unhealthy research behavior that is hard to eradicate
BACKGROUND: According to the CONSORT statement, significance testing of baseline differences in randomized controlled trials should not be performed. In fact, this practice has been discouraged by numerous authors throughout the last forty years. During that time span, reporting of baseline differen...
Autores principales: | , , , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
BioMed Central
2015
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4310023/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25616598 http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s12966-015-0162-z |
_version_ | 1782354790087393280 |
---|---|
author | de Boer, Michiel R Waterlander, Wilma E Kuijper, Lothar DJ Steenhuis, Ingrid HM Twisk, Jos WR |
author_facet | de Boer, Michiel R Waterlander, Wilma E Kuijper, Lothar DJ Steenhuis, Ingrid HM Twisk, Jos WR |
author_sort | de Boer, Michiel R |
collection | PubMed |
description | BACKGROUND: According to the CONSORT statement, significance testing of baseline differences in randomized controlled trials should not be performed. In fact, this practice has been discouraged by numerous authors throughout the last forty years. During that time span, reporting of baseline differences has substantially decreased in the leading general medical journals. Our own experience in the field of nutrition behavior research however, is that co-authors, reviewers and even editors are still very persistent in their demand for these tests. The aim of this paper is therefore to negate this demand by providing clear evidence as to why testing for baseline differences between intervention groups statistically is superfluous and why such results should not be published. DISCUSSION: Testing for baseline differences is often propagated because of the belief that it shows whether randomization was successful and it identifies real or important differences between treatment arms that should be accounted for in the statistical analyses. Especially the latter argument is flawed, because it ignores the fact that the prognostic strength of a variable is also important when the interest is in adjustment for confounding. In addition, including prognostic variables as covariates can increase the precision of the effect estimate. This means that choosing covariates based on significance tests for baseline differences might lead to omissions of important covariates and, less importantly, to inclusion of irrelevant covariates in the analysis. We used data from four supermarket trials on the effects of pricing strategies on fruit and vegetables purchases, to show that results from fully adjusted analyses sometimes do appreciably differ from results from analyses adjusted for significant baseline differences only. We propose to adjust for known or anticipated important prognostic variables. These could or should be pre-specified in trial protocols. Subsequently, authors should report results from the fully adjusted as well as crude analyses, especially for dichotomous and time to event data. SUMMARY: Based on our arguments, which were illustrated by our findings, we propose that journals in and outside the field of nutrition behavior actively adopt the CONSORT 2010 statement on this topic by not publishing significance tests for baseline differences anymore. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-4310023 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2015 |
publisher | BioMed Central |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-43100232015-01-30 Testing for baseline differences in randomized controlled trials: an unhealthy research behavior that is hard to eradicate de Boer, Michiel R Waterlander, Wilma E Kuijper, Lothar DJ Steenhuis, Ingrid HM Twisk, Jos WR Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act Debate BACKGROUND: According to the CONSORT statement, significance testing of baseline differences in randomized controlled trials should not be performed. In fact, this practice has been discouraged by numerous authors throughout the last forty years. During that time span, reporting of baseline differences has substantially decreased in the leading general medical journals. Our own experience in the field of nutrition behavior research however, is that co-authors, reviewers and even editors are still very persistent in their demand for these tests. The aim of this paper is therefore to negate this demand by providing clear evidence as to why testing for baseline differences between intervention groups statistically is superfluous and why such results should not be published. DISCUSSION: Testing for baseline differences is often propagated because of the belief that it shows whether randomization was successful and it identifies real or important differences between treatment arms that should be accounted for in the statistical analyses. Especially the latter argument is flawed, because it ignores the fact that the prognostic strength of a variable is also important when the interest is in adjustment for confounding. In addition, including prognostic variables as covariates can increase the precision of the effect estimate. This means that choosing covariates based on significance tests for baseline differences might lead to omissions of important covariates and, less importantly, to inclusion of irrelevant covariates in the analysis. We used data from four supermarket trials on the effects of pricing strategies on fruit and vegetables purchases, to show that results from fully adjusted analyses sometimes do appreciably differ from results from analyses adjusted for significant baseline differences only. We propose to adjust for known or anticipated important prognostic variables. These could or should be pre-specified in trial protocols. Subsequently, authors should report results from the fully adjusted as well as crude analyses, especially for dichotomous and time to event data. SUMMARY: Based on our arguments, which were illustrated by our findings, we propose that journals in and outside the field of nutrition behavior actively adopt the CONSORT 2010 statement on this topic by not publishing significance tests for baseline differences anymore. BioMed Central 2015-01-24 /pmc/articles/PMC4310023/ /pubmed/25616598 http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s12966-015-0162-z Text en © de Boer et al.; licensee BioMed Central. 2015 This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly credited. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated. |
spellingShingle | Debate de Boer, Michiel R Waterlander, Wilma E Kuijper, Lothar DJ Steenhuis, Ingrid HM Twisk, Jos WR Testing for baseline differences in randomized controlled trials: an unhealthy research behavior that is hard to eradicate |
title | Testing for baseline differences in randomized controlled trials: an unhealthy research behavior that is hard to eradicate |
title_full | Testing for baseline differences in randomized controlled trials: an unhealthy research behavior that is hard to eradicate |
title_fullStr | Testing for baseline differences in randomized controlled trials: an unhealthy research behavior that is hard to eradicate |
title_full_unstemmed | Testing for baseline differences in randomized controlled trials: an unhealthy research behavior that is hard to eradicate |
title_short | Testing for baseline differences in randomized controlled trials: an unhealthy research behavior that is hard to eradicate |
title_sort | testing for baseline differences in randomized controlled trials: an unhealthy research behavior that is hard to eradicate |
topic | Debate |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4310023/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25616598 http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s12966-015-0162-z |
work_keys_str_mv | AT deboermichielr testingforbaselinedifferencesinrandomizedcontrolledtrialsanunhealthyresearchbehaviorthatishardtoeradicate AT waterlanderwilmae testingforbaselinedifferencesinrandomizedcontrolledtrialsanunhealthyresearchbehaviorthatishardtoeradicate AT kuijperlothardj testingforbaselinedifferencesinrandomizedcontrolledtrialsanunhealthyresearchbehaviorthatishardtoeradicate AT steenhuisingridhm testingforbaselinedifferencesinrandomizedcontrolledtrialsanunhealthyresearchbehaviorthatishardtoeradicate AT twiskjoswr testingforbaselinedifferencesinrandomizedcontrolledtrialsanunhealthyresearchbehaviorthatishardtoeradicate |