Cargando…

Differences in neurocognitive aspects of dyslexia in Dutch and immigrant 6-7- and 8-9-years old children

Detecting dyslexia in immigrant children can be jeopardized because of assessment bias, as a consequence of a limited word lexicon or differences in language development of these children. This is in contrast with the view of the universal neurocognitive basis for dyslexia. In this research, differe...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Verpalen, Johanna MP, van de Vijver, Fons JR
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: Springer International Publishing 2015
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4356678/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25789208
http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s40064-015-0874-1
_version_ 1782361038779318272
author Verpalen, Johanna MP
van de Vijver, Fons JR
author_facet Verpalen, Johanna MP
van de Vijver, Fons JR
author_sort Verpalen, Johanna MP
collection PubMed
description Detecting dyslexia in immigrant children can be jeopardized because of assessment bias, as a consequence of a limited word lexicon or differences in language development of these children. This is in contrast with the view of the universal neurocognitive basis for dyslexia. In this research, differences in screening children at risk for dyslexia with the Dyslexia Screening Test (DST) were studied in third and fifth graders of primary school of Dutch (mainstream) and immigrant descent. Mean group differences were found on a few subtests (Naming Letters, Semantic Fluency, Backward Digit Span and Verbal Fluency), probably as a consequence of bias because of the linguistic character of these subtests. The raw scores of word lexicon increased in the Dutch and immigrant group. The association of having a dyslexia diagnosis on DST scores was comparable for Dutch and immigrant children. Differences in the DST scores between non-dyslexic and dyslexic children were found between the third and fifth grade, with a stronger effect of having a dyslexia diagnosis in the fifth grade than the third grade, for Dutch as well as immigrant children. Screening of dyslexia seems easier in the fifth grade than in the third grade, dyslexic children show a slower reading development than their non-dyslexic peers, irrespective of their cultural background. ELECTRONIC SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL: The online version of this article (doi:10.1186/s40064-015-0874-1) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-4356678
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2015
publisher Springer International Publishing
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-43566782015-03-18 Differences in neurocognitive aspects of dyslexia in Dutch and immigrant 6-7- and 8-9-years old children Verpalen, Johanna MP van de Vijver, Fons JR Springerplus Research Detecting dyslexia in immigrant children can be jeopardized because of assessment bias, as a consequence of a limited word lexicon or differences in language development of these children. This is in contrast with the view of the universal neurocognitive basis for dyslexia. In this research, differences in screening children at risk for dyslexia with the Dyslexia Screening Test (DST) were studied in third and fifth graders of primary school of Dutch (mainstream) and immigrant descent. Mean group differences were found on a few subtests (Naming Letters, Semantic Fluency, Backward Digit Span and Verbal Fluency), probably as a consequence of bias because of the linguistic character of these subtests. The raw scores of word lexicon increased in the Dutch and immigrant group. The association of having a dyslexia diagnosis on DST scores was comparable for Dutch and immigrant children. Differences in the DST scores between non-dyslexic and dyslexic children were found between the third and fifth grade, with a stronger effect of having a dyslexia diagnosis in the fifth grade than the third grade, for Dutch as well as immigrant children. Screening of dyslexia seems easier in the fifth grade than in the third grade, dyslexic children show a slower reading development than their non-dyslexic peers, irrespective of their cultural background. ELECTRONIC SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL: The online version of this article (doi:10.1186/s40064-015-0874-1) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users. Springer International Publishing 2015-03-01 /pmc/articles/PMC4356678/ /pubmed/25789208 http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s40064-015-0874-1 Text en © Verpalen and van de Vijver; licensee Springer. 2015 This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly credited.
spellingShingle Research
Verpalen, Johanna MP
van de Vijver, Fons JR
Differences in neurocognitive aspects of dyslexia in Dutch and immigrant 6-7- and 8-9-years old children
title Differences in neurocognitive aspects of dyslexia in Dutch and immigrant 6-7- and 8-9-years old children
title_full Differences in neurocognitive aspects of dyslexia in Dutch and immigrant 6-7- and 8-9-years old children
title_fullStr Differences in neurocognitive aspects of dyslexia in Dutch and immigrant 6-7- and 8-9-years old children
title_full_unstemmed Differences in neurocognitive aspects of dyslexia in Dutch and immigrant 6-7- and 8-9-years old children
title_short Differences in neurocognitive aspects of dyslexia in Dutch and immigrant 6-7- and 8-9-years old children
title_sort differences in neurocognitive aspects of dyslexia in dutch and immigrant 6-7- and 8-9-years old children
topic Research
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4356678/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25789208
http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s40064-015-0874-1
work_keys_str_mv AT verpalenjohannamp differencesinneurocognitiveaspectsofdyslexiaindutchandimmigrant67and89yearsoldchildren
AT vandevijverfonsjr differencesinneurocognitiveaspectsofdyslexiaindutchandimmigrant67and89yearsoldchildren