Cargando…
If so many are “few,” how few are “many”?
The scope of reference of a word's meaning can be highly variable. We present a novel paradigm to investigate the flexible interpretation of word meaning. We focus on quantifiers such as “many” or “few,” a class of words that depends on number knowledge but can be interpreted in a flexible mann...
Autores principales: | , , , , , , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
Frontiers Media S.A.
2015
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4400858/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25941502 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00441 |
_version_ | 1782367077451956224 |
---|---|
author | Heim, Stefan McMillan, Corey T. Clark, Robin Golob, Stephanie Min, Nam E. Olm, Christopher Powers, John Grossman, Murray |
author_facet | Heim, Stefan McMillan, Corey T. Clark, Robin Golob, Stephanie Min, Nam E. Olm, Christopher Powers, John Grossman, Murray |
author_sort | Heim, Stefan |
collection | PubMed |
description | The scope of reference of a word's meaning can be highly variable. We present a novel paradigm to investigate the flexible interpretation of word meaning. We focus on quantifiers such as “many” or “few,” a class of words that depends on number knowledge but can be interpreted in a flexible manner. Healthy young adults performed a truth value judgment task on pictorial arrays of varying amounts of blue and yellow circles, deciding whether the sentence “Many/few of the circles are yellow” was an adequate description of the stimulus. The study consisted of two experiments, one focusing on “many,” one on “few.” Each experiment had three blocks. In a first “baseline” block, each individual's criterion for “many” and “few” was assessed. In a second “adaptation” block, subjects received feedback about their decisions that was different from their initial judgments in an effort to evaluate the flexibility of a subject's interpretation. A third “test” block assessed whether adaptation of quantifier meaning induced in block 2 then was generalized to alter a subject's baseline meaning for “many” and “few.” In Experiment 1, a proportion of yellow circles as small as 40% was reinforced as “many”; in Experiment 2, a proportion of yellow circles as large as 60% was reinforced as “few.” Subjects learned the new criterion for “many” in Experiment 1, which also affected their criterion for “few” although it had never been mentioned. Likewise, in Experiment 2, subjects changed their criterion for “few,” with a comparable effect on the criterion for “many” which was not mentioned. Thus, the meaning of relational quantifiers like “many” and “few” is flexible and can be adapted. Most importantly, adapting the criterion for one quantifier (e.g., “many”) also appeared to affect the reciprocal quantifier (in this case, “few”). Implications of this result for psychological interventions and for investigations of the neurobiology of the language-number interface are discussed. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-4400858 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2015 |
publisher | Frontiers Media S.A. |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-44008582015-05-04 If so many are “few,” how few are “many”? Heim, Stefan McMillan, Corey T. Clark, Robin Golob, Stephanie Min, Nam E. Olm, Christopher Powers, John Grossman, Murray Front Psychol Psychology The scope of reference of a word's meaning can be highly variable. We present a novel paradigm to investigate the flexible interpretation of word meaning. We focus on quantifiers such as “many” or “few,” a class of words that depends on number knowledge but can be interpreted in a flexible manner. Healthy young adults performed a truth value judgment task on pictorial arrays of varying amounts of blue and yellow circles, deciding whether the sentence “Many/few of the circles are yellow” was an adequate description of the stimulus. The study consisted of two experiments, one focusing on “many,” one on “few.” Each experiment had three blocks. In a first “baseline” block, each individual's criterion for “many” and “few” was assessed. In a second “adaptation” block, subjects received feedback about their decisions that was different from their initial judgments in an effort to evaluate the flexibility of a subject's interpretation. A third “test” block assessed whether adaptation of quantifier meaning induced in block 2 then was generalized to alter a subject's baseline meaning for “many” and “few.” In Experiment 1, a proportion of yellow circles as small as 40% was reinforced as “many”; in Experiment 2, a proportion of yellow circles as large as 60% was reinforced as “few.” Subjects learned the new criterion for “many” in Experiment 1, which also affected their criterion for “few” although it had never been mentioned. Likewise, in Experiment 2, subjects changed their criterion for “few,” with a comparable effect on the criterion for “many” which was not mentioned. Thus, the meaning of relational quantifiers like “many” and “few” is flexible and can be adapted. Most importantly, adapting the criterion for one quantifier (e.g., “many”) also appeared to affect the reciprocal quantifier (in this case, “few”). Implications of this result for psychological interventions and for investigations of the neurobiology of the language-number interface are discussed. Frontiers Media S.A. 2015-04-17 /pmc/articles/PMC4400858/ /pubmed/25941502 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00441 Text en Copyright © 2015 Heim, McMillan, Clark, Golob, Min, Olm, Powers and Grossman. http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms. |
spellingShingle | Psychology Heim, Stefan McMillan, Corey T. Clark, Robin Golob, Stephanie Min, Nam E. Olm, Christopher Powers, John Grossman, Murray If so many are “few,” how few are “many”? |
title | If so many are “few,” how few are “many”? |
title_full | If so many are “few,” how few are “many”? |
title_fullStr | If so many are “few,” how few are “many”? |
title_full_unstemmed | If so many are “few,” how few are “many”? |
title_short | If so many are “few,” how few are “many”? |
title_sort | if so many are “few,” how few are “many”? |
topic | Psychology |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4400858/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25941502 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00441 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT heimstefan ifsomanyarefewhowfewaremany AT mcmillancoreyt ifsomanyarefewhowfewaremany AT clarkrobin ifsomanyarefewhowfewaremany AT golobstephanie ifsomanyarefewhowfewaremany AT minname ifsomanyarefewhowfewaremany AT olmchristopher ifsomanyarefewhowfewaremany AT powersjohn ifsomanyarefewhowfewaremany AT grossmanmurray ifsomanyarefewhowfewaremany |