Cargando…

Four-Year-Olds Use a Mixture of Spatial Reference Frames

Keeping track of unseen objects is an important spatial skill. In order to do this, people must situate the object in terms of different frames of reference, including body position (egocentric frame of reference), landmarks in the surrounding environment (extrinsic frame reference), or other attach...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Negen, James, Nardini, Marko
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: Public Library of Science 2015
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4489865/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26133990
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0131984
_version_ 1782379433456304128
author Negen, James
Nardini, Marko
author_facet Negen, James
Nardini, Marko
author_sort Negen, James
collection PubMed
description Keeping track of unseen objects is an important spatial skill. In order to do this, people must situate the object in terms of different frames of reference, including body position (egocentric frame of reference), landmarks in the surrounding environment (extrinsic frame reference), or other attached features (intrinsic frame of reference). Nardini et al. hid a toy in one of 12 cups in front of children, turned the array when they were not looking, and then asked them to point to the cup with the toy. This forced children to use the intrinsic frame (information about the array of cups) to locate the hidden toy. Three-year-olds made systematic errors by using the wrong frame of reference, 4-year-olds were at chance, and only 5- and 6-year-olds were successful. Can we better understand the developmental change that takes place at four years? This paper uses a modelling approach to re-examine the data and distinguish three possible strategies that could lead to the previous results at four years: (1) Children were choosing cups randomly, (2) Children were pointing between the egocentric/extrinsic-cued location and the correct target, and (3) Children were pointing near the egocentric/extrinsic-cued location on some trials and near the target on the rest. Results heavily favor the last possibility: 4-year-olds were not just guessing or trying to combine the available frames of reference. They were using the intrinsic frame on some trials, but not doing so consistently. These insights suggest that accounts of improving spatial performance at 4 years need to explain why there is a mixture of responses. Further application of the selected model also suggests that children become both more reliant on the correct frame and more accurate with any chosen frame as they mature.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-4489865
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2015
publisher Public Library of Science
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-44898652015-07-15 Four-Year-Olds Use a Mixture of Spatial Reference Frames Negen, James Nardini, Marko PLoS One Research Article Keeping track of unseen objects is an important spatial skill. In order to do this, people must situate the object in terms of different frames of reference, including body position (egocentric frame of reference), landmarks in the surrounding environment (extrinsic frame reference), or other attached features (intrinsic frame of reference). Nardini et al. hid a toy in one of 12 cups in front of children, turned the array when they were not looking, and then asked them to point to the cup with the toy. This forced children to use the intrinsic frame (information about the array of cups) to locate the hidden toy. Three-year-olds made systematic errors by using the wrong frame of reference, 4-year-olds were at chance, and only 5- and 6-year-olds were successful. Can we better understand the developmental change that takes place at four years? This paper uses a modelling approach to re-examine the data and distinguish three possible strategies that could lead to the previous results at four years: (1) Children were choosing cups randomly, (2) Children were pointing between the egocentric/extrinsic-cued location and the correct target, and (3) Children were pointing near the egocentric/extrinsic-cued location on some trials and near the target on the rest. Results heavily favor the last possibility: 4-year-olds were not just guessing or trying to combine the available frames of reference. They were using the intrinsic frame on some trials, but not doing so consistently. These insights suggest that accounts of improving spatial performance at 4 years need to explain why there is a mixture of responses. Further application of the selected model also suggests that children become both more reliant on the correct frame and more accurate with any chosen frame as they mature. Public Library of Science 2015-07-02 /pmc/articles/PMC4489865/ /pubmed/26133990 http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0131984 Text en © 2015 Negen, Nardini http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are properly credited.
spellingShingle Research Article
Negen, James
Nardini, Marko
Four-Year-Olds Use a Mixture of Spatial Reference Frames
title Four-Year-Olds Use a Mixture of Spatial Reference Frames
title_full Four-Year-Olds Use a Mixture of Spatial Reference Frames
title_fullStr Four-Year-Olds Use a Mixture of Spatial Reference Frames
title_full_unstemmed Four-Year-Olds Use a Mixture of Spatial Reference Frames
title_short Four-Year-Olds Use a Mixture of Spatial Reference Frames
title_sort four-year-olds use a mixture of spatial reference frames
topic Research Article
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4489865/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26133990
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0131984
work_keys_str_mv AT negenjames fouryearoldsuseamixtureofspatialreferenceframes
AT nardinimarko fouryearoldsuseamixtureofspatialreferenceframes