Cargando…
Functional flexibility in wild bonobo vocal behaviour
A shared principle in the evolution of language and the development of speech is the emergence of functional flexibility, the capacity of vocal signals to express a range of emotional states independently of context and biological function. Functional flexibility has recently been demonstrated in th...
Autores principales: | , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
PeerJ Inc.
2015
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4540007/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26290789 http://dx.doi.org/10.7717/peerj.1124 |
_version_ | 1782386180396941312 |
---|---|
author | Clay, Zanna Archbold, Jahmaira Zuberbühler, Klaus |
author_facet | Clay, Zanna Archbold, Jahmaira Zuberbühler, Klaus |
author_sort | Clay, Zanna |
collection | PubMed |
description | A shared principle in the evolution of language and the development of speech is the emergence of functional flexibility, the capacity of vocal signals to express a range of emotional states independently of context and biological function. Functional flexibility has recently been demonstrated in the vocalisations of pre-linguistic human infants, which has been contrasted to the functionally fixed vocal behaviour of non-human primates. Here, we revisited the presumed chasm in functional flexibility between human and non-human primate vocal behaviour, with a study on our closest living primate relatives, the bonobo (Pan paniscus). We found that wild bonobos use a specific call type (the “peep”) across a range of contexts that cover the full valence range (positive-neutral-negative) in much of their daily activities, including feeding, travel, rest, aggression, alarm, nesting and grooming. Peeps were produced in functionally flexible ways in some contexts, but not others. Crucially, calls did not vary acoustically between neutral and positive contexts, suggesting that recipients take pragmatic information into account to make inferences about call meaning. In comparison, peeps during negative contexts were acoustically distinct. Our data suggest that the capacity for functional flexibility has evolutionary roots that predate the evolution of human speech. We interpret this evidence as an example of an evolutionary early transition away from fixed vocal signalling towards functional flexibility. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-4540007 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2015 |
publisher | PeerJ Inc. |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-45400072015-08-19 Functional flexibility in wild bonobo vocal behaviour Clay, Zanna Archbold, Jahmaira Zuberbühler, Klaus PeerJ Animal Behavior A shared principle in the evolution of language and the development of speech is the emergence of functional flexibility, the capacity of vocal signals to express a range of emotional states independently of context and biological function. Functional flexibility has recently been demonstrated in the vocalisations of pre-linguistic human infants, which has been contrasted to the functionally fixed vocal behaviour of non-human primates. Here, we revisited the presumed chasm in functional flexibility between human and non-human primate vocal behaviour, with a study on our closest living primate relatives, the bonobo (Pan paniscus). We found that wild bonobos use a specific call type (the “peep”) across a range of contexts that cover the full valence range (positive-neutral-negative) in much of their daily activities, including feeding, travel, rest, aggression, alarm, nesting and grooming. Peeps were produced in functionally flexible ways in some contexts, but not others. Crucially, calls did not vary acoustically between neutral and positive contexts, suggesting that recipients take pragmatic information into account to make inferences about call meaning. In comparison, peeps during negative contexts were acoustically distinct. Our data suggest that the capacity for functional flexibility has evolutionary roots that predate the evolution of human speech. We interpret this evidence as an example of an evolutionary early transition away from fixed vocal signalling towards functional flexibility. PeerJ Inc. 2015-08-04 /pmc/articles/PMC4540007/ /pubmed/26290789 http://dx.doi.org/10.7717/peerj.1124 Text en © 2015 Clay et al. http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) , which permits unrestricted use, distribution, reproduction and adaptation in any medium and for any purpose provided that it is properly attributed. For attribution, the original author(s), title, publication source (PeerJ) and either DOI or URL of the article must be cited. |
spellingShingle | Animal Behavior Clay, Zanna Archbold, Jahmaira Zuberbühler, Klaus Functional flexibility in wild bonobo vocal behaviour |
title | Functional flexibility in wild bonobo vocal behaviour |
title_full | Functional flexibility in wild bonobo vocal behaviour |
title_fullStr | Functional flexibility in wild bonobo vocal behaviour |
title_full_unstemmed | Functional flexibility in wild bonobo vocal behaviour |
title_short | Functional flexibility in wild bonobo vocal behaviour |
title_sort | functional flexibility in wild bonobo vocal behaviour |
topic | Animal Behavior |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4540007/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26290789 http://dx.doi.org/10.7717/peerj.1124 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT clayzanna functionalflexibilityinwildbonobovocalbehaviour AT archboldjahmaira functionalflexibilityinwildbonobovocalbehaviour AT zuberbuhlerklaus functionalflexibilityinwildbonobovocalbehaviour |