Cargando…

Functional flexibility in wild bonobo vocal behaviour

A shared principle in the evolution of language and the development of speech is the emergence of functional flexibility, the capacity of vocal signals to express a range of emotional states independently of context and biological function. Functional flexibility has recently been demonstrated in th...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Clay, Zanna, Archbold, Jahmaira, Zuberbühler, Klaus
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: PeerJ Inc. 2015
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4540007/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26290789
http://dx.doi.org/10.7717/peerj.1124
_version_ 1782386180396941312
author Clay, Zanna
Archbold, Jahmaira
Zuberbühler, Klaus
author_facet Clay, Zanna
Archbold, Jahmaira
Zuberbühler, Klaus
author_sort Clay, Zanna
collection PubMed
description A shared principle in the evolution of language and the development of speech is the emergence of functional flexibility, the capacity of vocal signals to express a range of emotional states independently of context and biological function. Functional flexibility has recently been demonstrated in the vocalisations of pre-linguistic human infants, which has been contrasted to the functionally fixed vocal behaviour of non-human primates. Here, we revisited the presumed chasm in functional flexibility between human and non-human primate vocal behaviour, with a study on our closest living primate relatives, the bonobo (Pan paniscus). We found that wild bonobos use a specific call type (the “peep”) across a range of contexts that cover the full valence range (positive-neutral-negative) in much of their daily activities, including feeding, travel, rest, aggression, alarm, nesting and grooming. Peeps were produced in functionally flexible ways in some contexts, but not others. Crucially, calls did not vary acoustically between neutral and positive contexts, suggesting that recipients take pragmatic information into account to make inferences about call meaning. In comparison, peeps during negative contexts were acoustically distinct. Our data suggest that the capacity for functional flexibility has evolutionary roots that predate the evolution of human speech. We interpret this evidence as an example of an evolutionary early transition away from fixed vocal signalling towards functional flexibility.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-4540007
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2015
publisher PeerJ Inc.
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-45400072015-08-19 Functional flexibility in wild bonobo vocal behaviour Clay, Zanna Archbold, Jahmaira Zuberbühler, Klaus PeerJ Animal Behavior A shared principle in the evolution of language and the development of speech is the emergence of functional flexibility, the capacity of vocal signals to express a range of emotional states independently of context and biological function. Functional flexibility has recently been demonstrated in the vocalisations of pre-linguistic human infants, which has been contrasted to the functionally fixed vocal behaviour of non-human primates. Here, we revisited the presumed chasm in functional flexibility between human and non-human primate vocal behaviour, with a study on our closest living primate relatives, the bonobo (Pan paniscus). We found that wild bonobos use a specific call type (the “peep”) across a range of contexts that cover the full valence range (positive-neutral-negative) in much of their daily activities, including feeding, travel, rest, aggression, alarm, nesting and grooming. Peeps were produced in functionally flexible ways in some contexts, but not others. Crucially, calls did not vary acoustically between neutral and positive contexts, suggesting that recipients take pragmatic information into account to make inferences about call meaning. In comparison, peeps during negative contexts were acoustically distinct. Our data suggest that the capacity for functional flexibility has evolutionary roots that predate the evolution of human speech. We interpret this evidence as an example of an evolutionary early transition away from fixed vocal signalling towards functional flexibility. PeerJ Inc. 2015-08-04 /pmc/articles/PMC4540007/ /pubmed/26290789 http://dx.doi.org/10.7717/peerj.1124 Text en © 2015 Clay et al. http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) , which permits unrestricted use, distribution, reproduction and adaptation in any medium and for any purpose provided that it is properly attributed. For attribution, the original author(s), title, publication source (PeerJ) and either DOI or URL of the article must be cited.
spellingShingle Animal Behavior
Clay, Zanna
Archbold, Jahmaira
Zuberbühler, Klaus
Functional flexibility in wild bonobo vocal behaviour
title Functional flexibility in wild bonobo vocal behaviour
title_full Functional flexibility in wild bonobo vocal behaviour
title_fullStr Functional flexibility in wild bonobo vocal behaviour
title_full_unstemmed Functional flexibility in wild bonobo vocal behaviour
title_short Functional flexibility in wild bonobo vocal behaviour
title_sort functional flexibility in wild bonobo vocal behaviour
topic Animal Behavior
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4540007/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26290789
http://dx.doi.org/10.7717/peerj.1124
work_keys_str_mv AT clayzanna functionalflexibilityinwildbonobovocalbehaviour
AT archboldjahmaira functionalflexibilityinwildbonobovocalbehaviour
AT zuberbuhlerklaus functionalflexibilityinwildbonobovocalbehaviour