Cargando…

Texting at the light and other forms of device distraction behind the wheel

BACKGROUND: Cell phones are a well-known source of distraction for drivers, and owing to the proliferation of text messaging services, web browsers and interactive apps, modern devices provide ever-increasing temptation for drivers to take their eyes off the road. Although it is probably obvious tha...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Bernstein, James J., Bernstein, Joseph
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: BioMed Central 2015
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4584002/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26407864
http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s12889-015-2343-8
_version_ 1782391919856320512
author Bernstein, James J.
Bernstein, Joseph
author_facet Bernstein, James J.
Bernstein, Joseph
author_sort Bernstein, James J.
collection PubMed
description BACKGROUND: Cell phones are a well-known source of distraction for drivers, and owing to the proliferation of text messaging services, web browsers and interactive apps, modern devices provide ever-increasing temptation for drivers to take their eyes off the road. Although it is probably obvious that drivers’ manual engagement of a device while their vehicles are in motion is potentially dangerous, it may not be clear that such engagement when the vehicle is at rest (an activity broadly labeled “texting at the light”) can also impose risks. For one thing, a distracted driver at rest may fail to respond quickly to sudden changes in road conditions, such as an ambulance passing through. In addition, texting at the light may decrease so-called “situational awareness” and lead to driving errors even after the device is put down. To our knowledge, the direct comparison of the rate of device usage by drivers at rest with the rate of device usage by drivers in motion has not been reported. METHODS: We collected information on 2000 passenger vehicles by roadside observation. For the first group of 1000 passenger vehicles stopped at a traffic light, device usage (“texting”, “talking”, “none”), gender of the driver, vehicle type, seatbelt usage and presence of front seat passengers were recorded. For a second set of 1000 vehicles in motion, device usage alone was noted. Statistical significance for differences in rates was assessed with the chi-square test. RESULTS: We found that 3 % of drivers in motion were texting and 5 % were talking. Among the stopped drivers, 14.5 % were texting and 6.3 % were talking. In the stopped-vehicle set, gender and vehicle type were not associated with significant differences in device usage, but having a front seat passenger and using seatbelts were. CONCLUSIONS: Device usage is markedly higher among drivers temporarily at rest compared with those in motion, and the presence of a front seat passenger, who may help alleviate boredom or reprimand bad behavior, is associated with lower device usage rates among vehicles stopped at a light. These observations may help identify suitable steps to decrease distracted driving and thereby minimize traffic trauma.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-4584002
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2015
publisher BioMed Central
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-45840022015-09-28 Texting at the light and other forms of device distraction behind the wheel Bernstein, James J. Bernstein, Joseph BMC Public Health Research Article BACKGROUND: Cell phones are a well-known source of distraction for drivers, and owing to the proliferation of text messaging services, web browsers and interactive apps, modern devices provide ever-increasing temptation for drivers to take their eyes off the road. Although it is probably obvious that drivers’ manual engagement of a device while their vehicles are in motion is potentially dangerous, it may not be clear that such engagement when the vehicle is at rest (an activity broadly labeled “texting at the light”) can also impose risks. For one thing, a distracted driver at rest may fail to respond quickly to sudden changes in road conditions, such as an ambulance passing through. In addition, texting at the light may decrease so-called “situational awareness” and lead to driving errors even after the device is put down. To our knowledge, the direct comparison of the rate of device usage by drivers at rest with the rate of device usage by drivers in motion has not been reported. METHODS: We collected information on 2000 passenger vehicles by roadside observation. For the first group of 1000 passenger vehicles stopped at a traffic light, device usage (“texting”, “talking”, “none”), gender of the driver, vehicle type, seatbelt usage and presence of front seat passengers were recorded. For a second set of 1000 vehicles in motion, device usage alone was noted. Statistical significance for differences in rates was assessed with the chi-square test. RESULTS: We found that 3 % of drivers in motion were texting and 5 % were talking. Among the stopped drivers, 14.5 % were texting and 6.3 % were talking. In the stopped-vehicle set, gender and vehicle type were not associated with significant differences in device usage, but having a front seat passenger and using seatbelts were. CONCLUSIONS: Device usage is markedly higher among drivers temporarily at rest compared with those in motion, and the presence of a front seat passenger, who may help alleviate boredom or reprimand bad behavior, is associated with lower device usage rates among vehicles stopped at a light. These observations may help identify suitable steps to decrease distracted driving and thereby minimize traffic trauma. BioMed Central 2015-09-26 /pmc/articles/PMC4584002/ /pubmed/26407864 http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s12889-015-2343-8 Text en © Bernstein and Bernstein. 2015 Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.
spellingShingle Research Article
Bernstein, James J.
Bernstein, Joseph
Texting at the light and other forms of device distraction behind the wheel
title Texting at the light and other forms of device distraction behind the wheel
title_full Texting at the light and other forms of device distraction behind the wheel
title_fullStr Texting at the light and other forms of device distraction behind the wheel
title_full_unstemmed Texting at the light and other forms of device distraction behind the wheel
title_short Texting at the light and other forms of device distraction behind the wheel
title_sort texting at the light and other forms of device distraction behind the wheel
topic Research Article
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4584002/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26407864
http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s12889-015-2343-8
work_keys_str_mv AT bernsteinjamesj textingatthelightandotherformsofdevicedistractionbehindthewheel
AT bernsteinjoseph textingatthelightandotherformsofdevicedistractionbehindthewheel