Cargando…

Late Development of Cue Integration Is Linked to Sensory Fusion in Cortex

Adults optimize perceptual judgements by integrating different types of sensory information [1, 2]. This engages specialized neural circuits that fuse signals from the same [3, 4, 5] or different [6] modalities. Whereas young children can use sensory cues independently, adult-like precision gains fr...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Dekker, Tessa M., Ban, Hiroshi, van der Velde, Bauke, Sereno, Martin I., Welchman, Andrew E., Nardini, Marko
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: Cell Press 2015
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4635311/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26480841
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2015.09.043
_version_ 1782399483691139072
author Dekker, Tessa M.
Ban, Hiroshi
van der Velde, Bauke
Sereno, Martin I.
Welchman, Andrew E.
Nardini, Marko
author_facet Dekker, Tessa M.
Ban, Hiroshi
van der Velde, Bauke
Sereno, Martin I.
Welchman, Andrew E.
Nardini, Marko
author_sort Dekker, Tessa M.
collection PubMed
description Adults optimize perceptual judgements by integrating different types of sensory information [1, 2]. This engages specialized neural circuits that fuse signals from the same [3, 4, 5] or different [6] modalities. Whereas young children can use sensory cues independently, adult-like precision gains from cue combination only emerge around ages 10 to 11 years [7, 8, 9]. Why does it take so long to make best use of sensory information? Existing data cannot distinguish whether this (1) reflects surprisingly late changes in sensory processing (sensory integration mechanisms in the brain are still developing) or (2) depends on post-perceptual changes (integration in sensory cortex is adult-like, but higher-level decision processes do not access the information) [10]. We tested visual depth cue integration in the developing brain to distinguish these possibilities. We presented children aged 6–12 years with displays depicting depth from binocular disparity and relative motion and made measurements using psychophysics, retinotopic mapping, and pattern classification fMRI. Older children (>10.5 years) showed clear evidence for sensory fusion in V3B, a visual area thought to integrate depth cues in the adult brain [3, 4, 5]. By contrast, in younger children (<10.5 years), there was no evidence for sensory fusion in any visual area. This significant age difference was paired with a shift in perceptual performance around ages 10 to 11 years and could not be explained by motion artifacts, visual attention, or signal quality differences. Thus, whereas many basic visual processes mature early in childhood [11, 12], the brain circuits that fuse cues take a very long time to develop.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-4635311
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2015
publisher Cell Press
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-46353112015-12-01 Late Development of Cue Integration Is Linked to Sensory Fusion in Cortex Dekker, Tessa M. Ban, Hiroshi van der Velde, Bauke Sereno, Martin I. Welchman, Andrew E. Nardini, Marko Curr Biol Report Adults optimize perceptual judgements by integrating different types of sensory information [1, 2]. This engages specialized neural circuits that fuse signals from the same [3, 4, 5] or different [6] modalities. Whereas young children can use sensory cues independently, adult-like precision gains from cue combination only emerge around ages 10 to 11 years [7, 8, 9]. Why does it take so long to make best use of sensory information? Existing data cannot distinguish whether this (1) reflects surprisingly late changes in sensory processing (sensory integration mechanisms in the brain are still developing) or (2) depends on post-perceptual changes (integration in sensory cortex is adult-like, but higher-level decision processes do not access the information) [10]. We tested visual depth cue integration in the developing brain to distinguish these possibilities. We presented children aged 6–12 years with displays depicting depth from binocular disparity and relative motion and made measurements using psychophysics, retinotopic mapping, and pattern classification fMRI. Older children (>10.5 years) showed clear evidence for sensory fusion in V3B, a visual area thought to integrate depth cues in the adult brain [3, 4, 5]. By contrast, in younger children (<10.5 years), there was no evidence for sensory fusion in any visual area. This significant age difference was paired with a shift in perceptual performance around ages 10 to 11 years and could not be explained by motion artifacts, visual attention, or signal quality differences. Thus, whereas many basic visual processes mature early in childhood [11, 12], the brain circuits that fuse cues take a very long time to develop. Cell Press 2015-11-02 /pmc/articles/PMC4635311/ /pubmed/26480841 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2015.09.043 Text en © 2015 The Authors http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ This is an open access article under the CC BY license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
spellingShingle Report
Dekker, Tessa M.
Ban, Hiroshi
van der Velde, Bauke
Sereno, Martin I.
Welchman, Andrew E.
Nardini, Marko
Late Development of Cue Integration Is Linked to Sensory Fusion in Cortex
title Late Development of Cue Integration Is Linked to Sensory Fusion in Cortex
title_full Late Development of Cue Integration Is Linked to Sensory Fusion in Cortex
title_fullStr Late Development of Cue Integration Is Linked to Sensory Fusion in Cortex
title_full_unstemmed Late Development of Cue Integration Is Linked to Sensory Fusion in Cortex
title_short Late Development of Cue Integration Is Linked to Sensory Fusion in Cortex
title_sort late development of cue integration is linked to sensory fusion in cortex
topic Report
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4635311/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26480841
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2015.09.043
work_keys_str_mv AT dekkertessam latedevelopmentofcueintegrationislinkedtosensoryfusionincortex
AT banhiroshi latedevelopmentofcueintegrationislinkedtosensoryfusionincortex
AT vanderveldebauke latedevelopmentofcueintegrationislinkedtosensoryfusionincortex
AT serenomartini latedevelopmentofcueintegrationislinkedtosensoryfusionincortex
AT welchmanandrewe latedevelopmentofcueintegrationislinkedtosensoryfusionincortex
AT nardinimarko latedevelopmentofcueintegrationislinkedtosensoryfusionincortex