Cargando…
Rapid Naming in Brazilian Students with Dyslexia and Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder
Introduction: The effective development of reading and writing skills requires the concerted action of several abilities, one of which is phonological processing. One of the main components of phonological processing is rapid automatized naming (RAN)—the ability to identify and recognize a given ite...
Autores principales: | , , , , , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
Frontiers Media S.A.
2016
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4726778/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26858672 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00021 |
_version_ | 1782411887169765376 |
---|---|
author | Alves, Luciana Mendonça Siqueira, Cláudia M. Ferreira, Maria do Carmo Mangelli Alves, Juliana Flores Mendonça Lodi, Débora F. Bicalho, Lorena Celeste, Letícia C. |
author_facet | Alves, Luciana Mendonça Siqueira, Cláudia M. Ferreira, Maria do Carmo Mangelli Alves, Juliana Flores Mendonça Lodi, Débora F. Bicalho, Lorena Celeste, Letícia C. |
author_sort | Alves, Luciana Mendonça |
collection | PubMed |
description | Introduction: The effective development of reading and writing skills requires the concerted action of several abilities, one of which is phonological processing. One of the main components of phonological processing is rapid automatized naming (RAN)—the ability to identify and recognize a given item by the activation and concomitant articulation of its name. Objective: To assess the RAN performance of schoolchildren with dyslexia and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) compared with their peers. Methods: In total, 70 schoolchildren aged between 8 and 11 years participated in the study. Of these, 16 children had a multiprofessional diagnosis of ADHD while 14 were diagnosed with dyslexia. Matched with these groups, 40 schoolchildren with no history of developmental impairments were also evaluated. The RAN test was administered to assess the length of time required to name a series of familiar visual stimuli. The statistical analysis was conducted using measures of descriptive statistics and the 2-sample t-test at the 5% significance level. Results: The performance of the group with dyslexia was inferior to that of the control group in all tasks and the ADHD group had inferior performance for color and letters-naming tasks. The schoolchildren with dyslexia and those with ADHD showed very similar response times. Age was an important variable to be analyzed separately. As they aged, children with typical language development had fast answers on colors and digits tasks while children with dyslexia or ADHD did not show improvement with age. Conclusions: The schoolchildren with dyslexia took longer to complete all tasks and ADHD took longer to complete digits and objects tasks in comparison to their peers with typical development. This ability tended to improve with age, which was not the case, however, with schoolchildren who had ADHD or dyslexia. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-4726778 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2016 |
publisher | Frontiers Media S.A. |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-47267782016-02-08 Rapid Naming in Brazilian Students with Dyslexia and Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Alves, Luciana Mendonça Siqueira, Cláudia M. Ferreira, Maria do Carmo Mangelli Alves, Juliana Flores Mendonça Lodi, Débora F. Bicalho, Lorena Celeste, Letícia C. Front Psychol Psychology Introduction: The effective development of reading and writing skills requires the concerted action of several abilities, one of which is phonological processing. One of the main components of phonological processing is rapid automatized naming (RAN)—the ability to identify and recognize a given item by the activation and concomitant articulation of its name. Objective: To assess the RAN performance of schoolchildren with dyslexia and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) compared with their peers. Methods: In total, 70 schoolchildren aged between 8 and 11 years participated in the study. Of these, 16 children had a multiprofessional diagnosis of ADHD while 14 were diagnosed with dyslexia. Matched with these groups, 40 schoolchildren with no history of developmental impairments were also evaluated. The RAN test was administered to assess the length of time required to name a series of familiar visual stimuli. The statistical analysis was conducted using measures of descriptive statistics and the 2-sample t-test at the 5% significance level. Results: The performance of the group with dyslexia was inferior to that of the control group in all tasks and the ADHD group had inferior performance for color and letters-naming tasks. The schoolchildren with dyslexia and those with ADHD showed very similar response times. Age was an important variable to be analyzed separately. As they aged, children with typical language development had fast answers on colors and digits tasks while children with dyslexia or ADHD did not show improvement with age. Conclusions: The schoolchildren with dyslexia took longer to complete all tasks and ADHD took longer to complete digits and objects tasks in comparison to their peers with typical development. This ability tended to improve with age, which was not the case, however, with schoolchildren who had ADHD or dyslexia. Frontiers Media S.A. 2016-01-26 /pmc/articles/PMC4726778/ /pubmed/26858672 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00021 Text en Copyright © 2016 Alves, Siqueira, Ferreira, Alves, Lodi, Bicalho and Celeste. http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms. |
spellingShingle | Psychology Alves, Luciana Mendonça Siqueira, Cláudia M. Ferreira, Maria do Carmo Mangelli Alves, Juliana Flores Mendonça Lodi, Débora F. Bicalho, Lorena Celeste, Letícia C. Rapid Naming in Brazilian Students with Dyslexia and Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder |
title | Rapid Naming in Brazilian Students with Dyslexia and Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder |
title_full | Rapid Naming in Brazilian Students with Dyslexia and Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder |
title_fullStr | Rapid Naming in Brazilian Students with Dyslexia and Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder |
title_full_unstemmed | Rapid Naming in Brazilian Students with Dyslexia and Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder |
title_short | Rapid Naming in Brazilian Students with Dyslexia and Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder |
title_sort | rapid naming in brazilian students with dyslexia and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder |
topic | Psychology |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4726778/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26858672 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00021 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT alveslucianamendonca rapidnaminginbrazilianstudentswithdyslexiaandattentiondeficithyperactivitydisorder AT siqueiraclaudiam rapidnaminginbrazilianstudentswithdyslexiaandattentiondeficithyperactivitydisorder AT ferreiramariadocarmomangelli rapidnaminginbrazilianstudentswithdyslexiaandattentiondeficithyperactivitydisorder AT alvesjulianafloresmendonca rapidnaminginbrazilianstudentswithdyslexiaandattentiondeficithyperactivitydisorder AT lodideboraf rapidnaminginbrazilianstudentswithdyslexiaandattentiondeficithyperactivitydisorder AT bicalholorena rapidnaminginbrazilianstudentswithdyslexiaandattentiondeficithyperactivitydisorder AT celesteleticiac rapidnaminginbrazilianstudentswithdyslexiaandattentiondeficithyperactivitydisorder |