Cargando…

One-by-One or All-at-Once? Self-Reporting Policies and Dishonesty

Organizational monitoring relies frequently on self-reports (e.g., work hours, progress reports, travel expenses). A “one-by-one” policy requires employees to submit a series of reports (e.g., daily or itemized reports). An “all-at-once” policy requires an overall report (e.g., an annual or an overv...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Rilke, Rainer M., Schurr, Amos, Barkan, Rachel, Shalvi, Shaul
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: Frontiers Media S.A. 2016
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4756120/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26924997
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00113
_version_ 1782416272054550528
author Rilke, Rainer M.
Schurr, Amos
Barkan, Rachel
Shalvi, Shaul
author_facet Rilke, Rainer M.
Schurr, Amos
Barkan, Rachel
Shalvi, Shaul
author_sort Rilke, Rainer M.
collection PubMed
description Organizational monitoring relies frequently on self-reports (e.g., work hours, progress reports, travel expenses). A “one-by-one” policy requires employees to submit a series of reports (e.g., daily or itemized reports). An “all-at-once” policy requires an overall report (e.g., an annual or an overview report). Both policies use people's self-reports to determine their pay, and both allow people to inflate their reports to get higher incentives, that is, to cheat. Objectively, people can cheat to the same extent under both reporting policies. However, the two policies differ in that the segmented one-by-one policy signals closer monitoring than the all-at-once policy. We suggest here that lie aversion may have a paradoxical effect on closer monitoring and lead people to cheat more. Specifically, reporting a series of segmented units of performance (allowing small lies) should lead to more cheating than a one-shot report of overall performance (that require one larger lie). Two surveys indicated that while people perceive the all-at-once policy as more trusting, they still expected people would be equally likely to cheat in both policies. An experiment tested the effects of the two reporting policies on cheating. The findings showed that contrary to the participants' intuition, but in line with research on lie aversion, the one-by-one policy resulted in more cheating than the all-at-once policy. Implications for future research and organization policy are discussed.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-4756120
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2016
publisher Frontiers Media S.A.
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-47561202016-02-26 One-by-One or All-at-Once? Self-Reporting Policies and Dishonesty Rilke, Rainer M. Schurr, Amos Barkan, Rachel Shalvi, Shaul Front Psychol Psychology Organizational monitoring relies frequently on self-reports (e.g., work hours, progress reports, travel expenses). A “one-by-one” policy requires employees to submit a series of reports (e.g., daily or itemized reports). An “all-at-once” policy requires an overall report (e.g., an annual or an overview report). Both policies use people's self-reports to determine their pay, and both allow people to inflate their reports to get higher incentives, that is, to cheat. Objectively, people can cheat to the same extent under both reporting policies. However, the two policies differ in that the segmented one-by-one policy signals closer monitoring than the all-at-once policy. We suggest here that lie aversion may have a paradoxical effect on closer monitoring and lead people to cheat more. Specifically, reporting a series of segmented units of performance (allowing small lies) should lead to more cheating than a one-shot report of overall performance (that require one larger lie). Two surveys indicated that while people perceive the all-at-once policy as more trusting, they still expected people would be equally likely to cheat in both policies. An experiment tested the effects of the two reporting policies on cheating. The findings showed that contrary to the participants' intuition, but in line with research on lie aversion, the one-by-one policy resulted in more cheating than the all-at-once policy. Implications for future research and organization policy are discussed. Frontiers Media S.A. 2016-02-17 /pmc/articles/PMC4756120/ /pubmed/26924997 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00113 Text en Copyright © 2016 Rilke, Schurr, Barkan and Shalvi. http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
spellingShingle Psychology
Rilke, Rainer M.
Schurr, Amos
Barkan, Rachel
Shalvi, Shaul
One-by-One or All-at-Once? Self-Reporting Policies and Dishonesty
title One-by-One or All-at-Once? Self-Reporting Policies and Dishonesty
title_full One-by-One or All-at-Once? Self-Reporting Policies and Dishonesty
title_fullStr One-by-One or All-at-Once? Self-Reporting Policies and Dishonesty
title_full_unstemmed One-by-One or All-at-Once? Self-Reporting Policies and Dishonesty
title_short One-by-One or All-at-Once? Self-Reporting Policies and Dishonesty
title_sort one-by-one or all-at-once? self-reporting policies and dishonesty
topic Psychology
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4756120/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26924997
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00113
work_keys_str_mv AT rilkerainerm onebyoneorallatonceselfreportingpoliciesanddishonesty
AT schurramos onebyoneorallatonceselfreportingpoliciesanddishonesty
AT barkanrachel onebyoneorallatonceselfreportingpoliciesanddishonesty
AT shalvishaul onebyoneorallatonceselfreportingpoliciesanddishonesty