Cargando…
The Egocentric Nature of Action-Sound Associations
Actions that produce sounds infuse our daily lives. Some of these sounds are a natural consequence of physical interactions (such as a clang resulting from dropping a pan), but others are artificially designed (such as a beep resulting from a keypress). Although the relationship between actions and...
Autores principales: | , , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
Frontiers Media S.A.
2016
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4763076/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26941686 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00231 |
_version_ | 1782417199891218432 |
---|---|
author | Navolio, Nicole Lemaitre, Guillaume Forget, Alain Heller, Laurie M. |
author_facet | Navolio, Nicole Lemaitre, Guillaume Forget, Alain Heller, Laurie M. |
author_sort | Navolio, Nicole |
collection | PubMed |
description | Actions that produce sounds infuse our daily lives. Some of these sounds are a natural consequence of physical interactions (such as a clang resulting from dropping a pan), but others are artificially designed (such as a beep resulting from a keypress). Although the relationship between actions and sounds has previously been examined, the frame of reference of these associations is still unknown, despite it being a fundamental property of a psychological representation. For example, when an association is created between a keypress and a tone, it is unclear whether the frame of reference is egocentric (gesture-sound association) or exocentric (key-sound association). This question is especially important for artificially created associations, which occur in technology that pairs sounds with actions, such as gestural interfaces, virtual or augmented reality, and simple buttons that produce tones. The frame of reference could directly influence the learnability, the ease of use, the extent of immersion, and many other factors of the interaction. To explore whether action-sound associations are egocentric or exocentric, an experiment was implemented using a computer keyboard’s number pad wherein moving a finger from one key to another produced a sound, thus creating an action-sound association. Half of the participants received egocentric instructions to move their finger with a particular gesture. The other half of the participants received exocentric instructions to move their finger to a particular number on the keypad. All participants were performing the same actions, and only the framing of the action varied between conditions by altering task instructions. Participants in the egocentric condition learned the gesture-sound association, as revealed by a priming paradigm. However, the exocentric condition showed no priming effects. This finding suggests that action-sound associations are egocentric in nature. A second part of the same session further confirmed the egocentric nature of these associations by showing no change in the priming effect after moving to a different starting location. Our findings are consistent with an egocentric representation of action-sound associations, which could have implications for applications that utilize these associations. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-4763076 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2016 |
publisher | Frontiers Media S.A. |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-47630762016-03-03 The Egocentric Nature of Action-Sound Associations Navolio, Nicole Lemaitre, Guillaume Forget, Alain Heller, Laurie M. Front Psychol Psychology Actions that produce sounds infuse our daily lives. Some of these sounds are a natural consequence of physical interactions (such as a clang resulting from dropping a pan), but others are artificially designed (such as a beep resulting from a keypress). Although the relationship between actions and sounds has previously been examined, the frame of reference of these associations is still unknown, despite it being a fundamental property of a psychological representation. For example, when an association is created between a keypress and a tone, it is unclear whether the frame of reference is egocentric (gesture-sound association) or exocentric (key-sound association). This question is especially important for artificially created associations, which occur in technology that pairs sounds with actions, such as gestural interfaces, virtual or augmented reality, and simple buttons that produce tones. The frame of reference could directly influence the learnability, the ease of use, the extent of immersion, and many other factors of the interaction. To explore whether action-sound associations are egocentric or exocentric, an experiment was implemented using a computer keyboard’s number pad wherein moving a finger from one key to another produced a sound, thus creating an action-sound association. Half of the participants received egocentric instructions to move their finger with a particular gesture. The other half of the participants received exocentric instructions to move their finger to a particular number on the keypad. All participants were performing the same actions, and only the framing of the action varied between conditions by altering task instructions. Participants in the egocentric condition learned the gesture-sound association, as revealed by a priming paradigm. However, the exocentric condition showed no priming effects. This finding suggests that action-sound associations are egocentric in nature. A second part of the same session further confirmed the egocentric nature of these associations by showing no change in the priming effect after moving to a different starting location. Our findings are consistent with an egocentric representation of action-sound associations, which could have implications for applications that utilize these associations. Frontiers Media S.A. 2016-02-23 /pmc/articles/PMC4763076/ /pubmed/26941686 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00231 Text en Copyright © 2016 Navolio, Lemaitre, Forget and Heller. http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms. |
spellingShingle | Psychology Navolio, Nicole Lemaitre, Guillaume Forget, Alain Heller, Laurie M. The Egocentric Nature of Action-Sound Associations |
title | The Egocentric Nature of Action-Sound Associations |
title_full | The Egocentric Nature of Action-Sound Associations |
title_fullStr | The Egocentric Nature of Action-Sound Associations |
title_full_unstemmed | The Egocentric Nature of Action-Sound Associations |
title_short | The Egocentric Nature of Action-Sound Associations |
title_sort | egocentric nature of action-sound associations |
topic | Psychology |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4763076/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26941686 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00231 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT navolionicole theegocentricnatureofactionsoundassociations AT lemaitreguillaume theegocentricnatureofactionsoundassociations AT forgetalain theegocentricnatureofactionsoundassociations AT hellerlauriem theegocentricnatureofactionsoundassociations AT navolionicole egocentricnatureofactionsoundassociations AT lemaitreguillaume egocentricnatureofactionsoundassociations AT forgetalain egocentricnatureofactionsoundassociations AT hellerlauriem egocentricnatureofactionsoundassociations |