Cargando…

Slowing after Observed Error Transfers across Tasks

After committing an error, participants tend to perform more slowly. This phenomenon is called post-error slowing (PES). Although previous studies have explored the PES effect in the context of observed errors, the issue as to whether the slowing effect generalizes across tasksets remains unclear. F...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Wang, Lijun, Pan, Weigang, Tan, Jinfeng, Liu, Congcong, Chen, Antao
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: Public Library of Science 2016
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4775031/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26934579
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0149836
_version_ 1782419011381755904
author Wang, Lijun
Pan, Weigang
Tan, Jinfeng
Liu, Congcong
Chen, Antao
author_facet Wang, Lijun
Pan, Weigang
Tan, Jinfeng
Liu, Congcong
Chen, Antao
author_sort Wang, Lijun
collection PubMed
description After committing an error, participants tend to perform more slowly. This phenomenon is called post-error slowing (PES). Although previous studies have explored the PES effect in the context of observed errors, the issue as to whether the slowing effect generalizes across tasksets remains unclear. Further, the generation mechanisms of PES following observed errors must be examined. To address the above issues, we employed an observation-execution task in three experiments. During each trial, participants were required to mentally observe the outcomes of their partners in the observation task and then to perform their own key-press according to the mapping rules in the execution task. In Experiment 1, the same tasksets were utilized in the observation task and the execution task, and three error rate conditions (20%, 50% and 80%) were established in the observation task. The results revealed that the PES effect after observed errors was obtained in all three error rate conditions, replicating and extending previous studies. In Experiment 2, distinct stimuli and response rules were utilized in the observation task and the execution task. The result pattern was the same as that in Experiment 1, suggesting that the PES effect after observed errors was a generic adjustment process. In Experiment 3, the response deadline was shortened in the execution task to rule out the ceiling effect, and two error rate conditions (50% and 80%) were established in the observation task. The PES effect after observed errors was still obtained in the 50% and 80% error rate conditions. However, the accuracy in the post-observed error trials was comparable to that in the post-observed correct trials, suggesting that the slowing effect and improved accuracy did not rely on the same underlying mechanism. Current findings indicate that the occurrence of PES after observed errors is not dependent on the probability of observed errors, consistent with the assumption of cognitive control account. Moreover, the PES effect appears across tasksets with distinct stimuli and response rules in the context of observed errors, reflecting a generic process. Additionally, the slowing effect and improved accuracy in the post-observed error trial do not occur together, suggesting that they are independent behavioral adjustments in the context of observed errors.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-4775031
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2016
publisher Public Library of Science
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-47750312016-03-10 Slowing after Observed Error Transfers across Tasks Wang, Lijun Pan, Weigang Tan, Jinfeng Liu, Congcong Chen, Antao PLoS One Research Article After committing an error, participants tend to perform more slowly. This phenomenon is called post-error slowing (PES). Although previous studies have explored the PES effect in the context of observed errors, the issue as to whether the slowing effect generalizes across tasksets remains unclear. Further, the generation mechanisms of PES following observed errors must be examined. To address the above issues, we employed an observation-execution task in three experiments. During each trial, participants were required to mentally observe the outcomes of their partners in the observation task and then to perform their own key-press according to the mapping rules in the execution task. In Experiment 1, the same tasksets were utilized in the observation task and the execution task, and three error rate conditions (20%, 50% and 80%) were established in the observation task. The results revealed that the PES effect after observed errors was obtained in all three error rate conditions, replicating and extending previous studies. In Experiment 2, distinct stimuli and response rules were utilized in the observation task and the execution task. The result pattern was the same as that in Experiment 1, suggesting that the PES effect after observed errors was a generic adjustment process. In Experiment 3, the response deadline was shortened in the execution task to rule out the ceiling effect, and two error rate conditions (50% and 80%) were established in the observation task. The PES effect after observed errors was still obtained in the 50% and 80% error rate conditions. However, the accuracy in the post-observed error trials was comparable to that in the post-observed correct trials, suggesting that the slowing effect and improved accuracy did not rely on the same underlying mechanism. Current findings indicate that the occurrence of PES after observed errors is not dependent on the probability of observed errors, consistent with the assumption of cognitive control account. Moreover, the PES effect appears across tasksets with distinct stimuli and response rules in the context of observed errors, reflecting a generic process. Additionally, the slowing effect and improved accuracy in the post-observed error trial do not occur together, suggesting that they are independent behavioral adjustments in the context of observed errors. Public Library of Science 2016-03-02 /pmc/articles/PMC4775031/ /pubmed/26934579 http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0149836 Text en © 2016 Wang et al http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) , which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
spellingShingle Research Article
Wang, Lijun
Pan, Weigang
Tan, Jinfeng
Liu, Congcong
Chen, Antao
Slowing after Observed Error Transfers across Tasks
title Slowing after Observed Error Transfers across Tasks
title_full Slowing after Observed Error Transfers across Tasks
title_fullStr Slowing after Observed Error Transfers across Tasks
title_full_unstemmed Slowing after Observed Error Transfers across Tasks
title_short Slowing after Observed Error Transfers across Tasks
title_sort slowing after observed error transfers across tasks
topic Research Article
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4775031/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26934579
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0149836
work_keys_str_mv AT wanglijun slowingafterobservederrortransfersacrosstasks
AT panweigang slowingafterobservederrortransfersacrosstasks
AT tanjinfeng slowingafterobservederrortransfersacrosstasks
AT liucongcong slowingafterobservederrortransfersacrosstasks
AT chenantao slowingafterobservederrortransfersacrosstasks