Cargando…

Vergence Adaptation to Short-Duration Stimuli in Early Childhood

PURPOSE: To investigate whether nonstrabismic typically developing young children are capable of exhibiting vergence adaptation. METHODS: Fifteen adults (19.5–35.8 years) and 34 children (2.5–7.3 years) provided usable data. None wore habitual refractive correction. Eye position and accommodation we...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Babinsky, Erin, Sreenivasan, Vidhyapriya, Candy, T. Rowan
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: The Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology 2016
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4782828/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26943155
http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/iovs.15-17767
_version_ 1782420019070631936
author Babinsky, Erin
Sreenivasan, Vidhyapriya
Candy, T. Rowan
author_facet Babinsky, Erin
Sreenivasan, Vidhyapriya
Candy, T. Rowan
author_sort Babinsky, Erin
collection PubMed
description PURPOSE: To investigate whether nonstrabismic typically developing young children are capable of exhibiting vergence adaptation. METHODS: Fifteen adults (19.5–35.8 years) and 34 children (2.5–7.3 years) provided usable data. None wore habitual refractive correction. Eye position and accommodation were recorded using Purkinje image eye tracking and eccentric photorefraction (MCS PowerRefractor). Vergence was measured in three conditions while the participant viewed naturalistic targets at 33 cm. Viewing was monocular for at least 60 seconds and then binocular for either 5 seconds (5-second condition), 60 seconds (60-second), or 60 seconds through a 10-pd base-out prism (prism 60-second). The right eye was then occluded again for 60 seconds and an exponential function was fit to these data to assess the impact of adaptation on alignment. RESULTS: The 63% time constant was significantly longer for the prism 60-second condition (mean = 11.5 seconds) compared to both the 5-second (5.3 seconds; P = 0.015) and the 60-second conditions (7.1 seconds; P = 0.035), with no significant difference between children and adults (P > 0.4). Correlations between the 63% time constant (prism 60-second condition) and age, refractive error, interpupillary distance (IPD), or baseline heterophoria were not significant (P > 0.4). The final stable monocular alignment, measured after binocular viewing, was similar to the baseline initial alignment across all conditions and ages. CONCLUSIONS: For a limited-duration near task, 2- to 7-year-old children showed comparable levels of vergence adaptation to adults. In a typically developing visual system, where IPD and refractive error are maturing, this adaptation could help maintain eye alignment.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-4782828
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2016
publisher The Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-47828282016-09-01 Vergence Adaptation to Short-Duration Stimuli in Early Childhood Babinsky, Erin Sreenivasan, Vidhyapriya Candy, T. Rowan Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci Eye Movements, Strabismus, Amblyopia and Neuro-Ophthalmology PURPOSE: To investigate whether nonstrabismic typically developing young children are capable of exhibiting vergence adaptation. METHODS: Fifteen adults (19.5–35.8 years) and 34 children (2.5–7.3 years) provided usable data. None wore habitual refractive correction. Eye position and accommodation were recorded using Purkinje image eye tracking and eccentric photorefraction (MCS PowerRefractor). Vergence was measured in three conditions while the participant viewed naturalistic targets at 33 cm. Viewing was monocular for at least 60 seconds and then binocular for either 5 seconds (5-second condition), 60 seconds (60-second), or 60 seconds through a 10-pd base-out prism (prism 60-second). The right eye was then occluded again for 60 seconds and an exponential function was fit to these data to assess the impact of adaptation on alignment. RESULTS: The 63% time constant was significantly longer for the prism 60-second condition (mean = 11.5 seconds) compared to both the 5-second (5.3 seconds; P = 0.015) and the 60-second conditions (7.1 seconds; P = 0.035), with no significant difference between children and adults (P > 0.4). Correlations between the 63% time constant (prism 60-second condition) and age, refractive error, interpupillary distance (IPD), or baseline heterophoria were not significant (P > 0.4). The final stable monocular alignment, measured after binocular viewing, was similar to the baseline initial alignment across all conditions and ages. CONCLUSIONS: For a limited-duration near task, 2- to 7-year-old children showed comparable levels of vergence adaptation to adults. In a typically developing visual system, where IPD and refractive error are maturing, this adaptation could help maintain eye alignment. The Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology 2016-03-04 2016-03 /pmc/articles/PMC4782828/ /pubmed/26943155 http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/iovs.15-17767 Text en http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/ This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License.
spellingShingle Eye Movements, Strabismus, Amblyopia and Neuro-Ophthalmology
Babinsky, Erin
Sreenivasan, Vidhyapriya
Candy, T. Rowan
Vergence Adaptation to Short-Duration Stimuli in Early Childhood
title Vergence Adaptation to Short-Duration Stimuli in Early Childhood
title_full Vergence Adaptation to Short-Duration Stimuli in Early Childhood
title_fullStr Vergence Adaptation to Short-Duration Stimuli in Early Childhood
title_full_unstemmed Vergence Adaptation to Short-Duration Stimuli in Early Childhood
title_short Vergence Adaptation to Short-Duration Stimuli in Early Childhood
title_sort vergence adaptation to short-duration stimuli in early childhood
topic Eye Movements, Strabismus, Amblyopia and Neuro-Ophthalmology
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4782828/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26943155
http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/iovs.15-17767
work_keys_str_mv AT babinskyerin vergenceadaptationtoshortdurationstimuliinearlychildhood
AT sreenivasanvidhyapriya vergenceadaptationtoshortdurationstimuliinearlychildhood
AT candytrowan vergenceadaptationtoshortdurationstimuliinearlychildhood