Cargando…
The Stereoscopic Anisotropy Develops During Childhood
PURPOSE: Human vision has a puzzling stereoscopic anisotropy: horizontal depth corrugations are easier to detect than vertical depth corrugations. To date, little is known about the function or the underlying mechanism responsible for this anisotropy. Here, we aim to find out whether this anisotropy...
Autores principales: | , , , , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
The Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology
2016
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4788095/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26962692 http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/iovs.15-17766 |
_version_ | 1782420701379035136 |
---|---|
author | Serrano-Pedraza, Ignacio Herbert, William Villa-Laso, Laura Widdall, Michael Vancleef, Kathleen Read, Jenny C. A. |
author_facet | Serrano-Pedraza, Ignacio Herbert, William Villa-Laso, Laura Widdall, Michael Vancleef, Kathleen Read, Jenny C. A. |
author_sort | Serrano-Pedraza, Ignacio |
collection | PubMed |
description | PURPOSE: Human vision has a puzzling stereoscopic anisotropy: horizontal depth corrugations are easier to detect than vertical depth corrugations. To date, little is known about the function or the underlying mechanism responsible for this anisotropy. Here, we aim to find out whether this anisotropy is independent of age. To answer this, we compare detection thresholds for horizontal and vertical depth corrugations as a function of age. METHODS: The depth corrugations were defined solely by the horizontal disparity of random dot patterns. The disparities depicted a horizontal or vertical sinusoidal depth corrugation of spatial frequency 0.1 cyc/deg. Detection thresholds were obtained using Bayesian adaptive staircases from a total of 159 subjects aged from 3 to 73 years. For each participant we computed the anisotropy index, defined as the log(10)-ratio of the detection threshold for vertical corrugations divided by that for horizontal. RESULTS: Anisotropy index was highly variable between individuals but was positive in 87% of the participants. There was a significant correlation between anisotropy index and log-age (r = 0.21, P = 0.008) mainly driven by a significant difference between children and adults. In 67 children aged 3 to 13 years, the mean anisotropy index was 0.34 ± 0.38 (mean ± SD, meaning that vertical thresholds were on average 2.2 times the horizontal ones), compared with 0.59 ± 0.55 in 84 adults aged 18 to 73 years (vertical 3.9 times horizontal). This was mainly driven by a decline in the sensitivity to vertical corrugations. Children had poorer stereoacuity than adults, but had similar sensitivity to adults for horizontal corrugations and were actually more sensitive than adults to vertical corrugations. CONCLUSIONS: The fact that adults show stronger stereo anisotropy than children raises the possibility that visual experience plays a critical role in developing and strengthening the stereo anisotropy. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-4788095 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2016 |
publisher | The Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-47880952016-09-01 The Stereoscopic Anisotropy Develops During Childhood Serrano-Pedraza, Ignacio Herbert, William Villa-Laso, Laura Widdall, Michael Vancleef, Kathleen Read, Jenny C. A. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci Visual Psychophysics and Physiological Optics PURPOSE: Human vision has a puzzling stereoscopic anisotropy: horizontal depth corrugations are easier to detect than vertical depth corrugations. To date, little is known about the function or the underlying mechanism responsible for this anisotropy. Here, we aim to find out whether this anisotropy is independent of age. To answer this, we compare detection thresholds for horizontal and vertical depth corrugations as a function of age. METHODS: The depth corrugations were defined solely by the horizontal disparity of random dot patterns. The disparities depicted a horizontal or vertical sinusoidal depth corrugation of spatial frequency 0.1 cyc/deg. Detection thresholds were obtained using Bayesian adaptive staircases from a total of 159 subjects aged from 3 to 73 years. For each participant we computed the anisotropy index, defined as the log(10)-ratio of the detection threshold for vertical corrugations divided by that for horizontal. RESULTS: Anisotropy index was highly variable between individuals but was positive in 87% of the participants. There was a significant correlation between anisotropy index and log-age (r = 0.21, P = 0.008) mainly driven by a significant difference between children and adults. In 67 children aged 3 to 13 years, the mean anisotropy index was 0.34 ± 0.38 (mean ± SD, meaning that vertical thresholds were on average 2.2 times the horizontal ones), compared with 0.59 ± 0.55 in 84 adults aged 18 to 73 years (vertical 3.9 times horizontal). This was mainly driven by a decline in the sensitivity to vertical corrugations. Children had poorer stereoacuity than adults, but had similar sensitivity to adults for horizontal corrugations and were actually more sensitive than adults to vertical corrugations. CONCLUSIONS: The fact that adults show stronger stereo anisotropy than children raises the possibility that visual experience plays a critical role in developing and strengthening the stereo anisotropy. The Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology 2016-03-08 2016-03 /pmc/articles/PMC4788095/ /pubmed/26962692 http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/iovs.15-17766 Text en http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License. |
spellingShingle | Visual Psychophysics and Physiological Optics Serrano-Pedraza, Ignacio Herbert, William Villa-Laso, Laura Widdall, Michael Vancleef, Kathleen Read, Jenny C. A. The Stereoscopic Anisotropy Develops During Childhood |
title | The Stereoscopic Anisotropy Develops During Childhood |
title_full | The Stereoscopic Anisotropy Develops During Childhood |
title_fullStr | The Stereoscopic Anisotropy Develops During Childhood |
title_full_unstemmed | The Stereoscopic Anisotropy Develops During Childhood |
title_short | The Stereoscopic Anisotropy Develops During Childhood |
title_sort | stereoscopic anisotropy develops during childhood |
topic | Visual Psychophysics and Physiological Optics |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4788095/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26962692 http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/iovs.15-17766 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT serranopedrazaignacio thestereoscopicanisotropydevelopsduringchildhood AT herbertwilliam thestereoscopicanisotropydevelopsduringchildhood AT villalasolaura thestereoscopicanisotropydevelopsduringchildhood AT widdallmichael thestereoscopicanisotropydevelopsduringchildhood AT vancleefkathleen thestereoscopicanisotropydevelopsduringchildhood AT readjennyca thestereoscopicanisotropydevelopsduringchildhood AT serranopedrazaignacio stereoscopicanisotropydevelopsduringchildhood AT herbertwilliam stereoscopicanisotropydevelopsduringchildhood AT villalasolaura stereoscopicanisotropydevelopsduringchildhood AT widdallmichael stereoscopicanisotropydevelopsduringchildhood AT vancleefkathleen stereoscopicanisotropydevelopsduringchildhood AT readjennyca stereoscopicanisotropydevelopsduringchildhood |