Cargando…
Coercion Changes the Sense of Agency in the Human Brain
People may deny responsibility for negative consequences of their actions by claiming that they were “only obeying orders.” The “Nuremberg defense” offers one extreme example, though it is often dismissed as merely an attempt to avoid responsibility. Milgram’s classic laboratory studies reported wid...
Autores principales: | , , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
Cell Press
2016
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4791480/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26898470 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2015.12.067 |
_version_ | 1782421098020732928 |
---|---|
author | Caspar, Emilie A. Christensen, Julia F. Cleeremans, Axel Haggard, Patrick |
author_facet | Caspar, Emilie A. Christensen, Julia F. Cleeremans, Axel Haggard, Patrick |
author_sort | Caspar, Emilie A. |
collection | PubMed |
description | People may deny responsibility for negative consequences of their actions by claiming that they were “only obeying orders.” The “Nuremberg defense” offers one extreme example, though it is often dismissed as merely an attempt to avoid responsibility. Milgram’s classic laboratory studies reported widespread obedience to an instruction to harm, suggesting that social coercion may alter mechanisms of voluntary agency, and hence abolish the normal experience of being in control of one’s own actions. However, Milgram’s and other studies relied on dissembling and on explicit measures of agency, which are known to be biased by social norms. Here, we combined coercive instructions to administer harm to a co-participant, with implicit measures of sense of agency, based on perceived compression of time intervals between voluntary actions and their outcomes, and with electrophysiological recordings. In two experiments, an experimenter ordered a volunteer to make a key-press action that caused either financial penalty or demonstrably painful electric shock to their co-participant, thereby increasing their own financial gain. Coercion increased the perceived interval between action and outcome, relative to a situation where participants freely chose to inflict the same harms. Interestingly, coercion also reduced the neural processing of the outcomes of one’s own action. Thus, people who obey orders may subjectively experience their actions as closer to passive movements than fully voluntary actions. Our results highlight the complex relation between the brain mechanisms that generate the subjective experience of voluntary actions and social constructs, such as responsibility. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-4791480 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2016 |
publisher | Cell Press |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-47914802016-03-24 Coercion Changes the Sense of Agency in the Human Brain Caspar, Emilie A. Christensen, Julia F. Cleeremans, Axel Haggard, Patrick Curr Biol Article People may deny responsibility for negative consequences of their actions by claiming that they were “only obeying orders.” The “Nuremberg defense” offers one extreme example, though it is often dismissed as merely an attempt to avoid responsibility. Milgram’s classic laboratory studies reported widespread obedience to an instruction to harm, suggesting that social coercion may alter mechanisms of voluntary agency, and hence abolish the normal experience of being in control of one’s own actions. However, Milgram’s and other studies relied on dissembling and on explicit measures of agency, which are known to be biased by social norms. Here, we combined coercive instructions to administer harm to a co-participant, with implicit measures of sense of agency, based on perceived compression of time intervals between voluntary actions and their outcomes, and with electrophysiological recordings. In two experiments, an experimenter ordered a volunteer to make a key-press action that caused either financial penalty or demonstrably painful electric shock to their co-participant, thereby increasing their own financial gain. Coercion increased the perceived interval between action and outcome, relative to a situation where participants freely chose to inflict the same harms. Interestingly, coercion also reduced the neural processing of the outcomes of one’s own action. Thus, people who obey orders may subjectively experience their actions as closer to passive movements than fully voluntary actions. Our results highlight the complex relation between the brain mechanisms that generate the subjective experience of voluntary actions and social constructs, such as responsibility. Cell Press 2016-03-07 /pmc/articles/PMC4791480/ /pubmed/26898470 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2015.12.067 Text en © 2016 The Authors http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/ This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/). |
spellingShingle | Article Caspar, Emilie A. Christensen, Julia F. Cleeremans, Axel Haggard, Patrick Coercion Changes the Sense of Agency in the Human Brain |
title | Coercion Changes the Sense of Agency in the Human Brain |
title_full | Coercion Changes the Sense of Agency in the Human Brain |
title_fullStr | Coercion Changes the Sense of Agency in the Human Brain |
title_full_unstemmed | Coercion Changes the Sense of Agency in the Human Brain |
title_short | Coercion Changes the Sense of Agency in the Human Brain |
title_sort | coercion changes the sense of agency in the human brain |
topic | Article |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4791480/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26898470 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2015.12.067 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT casparemiliea coercionchangesthesenseofagencyinthehumanbrain AT christensenjuliaf coercionchangesthesenseofagencyinthehumanbrain AT cleeremansaxel coercionchangesthesenseofagencyinthehumanbrain AT haggardpatrick coercionchangesthesenseofagencyinthehumanbrain |